2000
DOI: 10.1073/pnas.050461497
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Learning improves growth rate in grasshoppers

Abstract: To quantify the adaptive significance of insect learning, we documented the behavior and growth rate of grasshoppers (Schistocerca americana) in an environment containing two artificial food types, one providing a balanced diet of protein and carbohydrate, which maximizes growth, and the other being carbohydratedeficient, which is unsuitable for growth. Grasshoppers in the Learning treatment experienced a predictable environment, where the spatial location, taste, and color of each food source remained constan… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

2
128
0
1

Year Published

2001
2001
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 176 publications
(131 citation statements)
references
References 20 publications
2
128
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…While it is intuitively appealing to assume that such variation in learning performance is adaptive ( Johnston 1982;Dukas 1998), few studies have yet been conducted to specifically examine this link under natural conditions. Laboratory studies, using grasshoppers (Dukas & Bernays 2000) and parasitoid wasps (Dukas & Duan 2000), suggest that animals able to form associations between cues (such as colour, odour or location) and rewards perform better than animals prevented from learning. Other laboratory studies, applying artificial selection to the learning ability of fruitflies, provide evidence for potential fitness costs associated with enhanced performance in associative learning (Mery & Kawecki 2003, 2004 or long-term memory (Mery & Kawecki 2005) tasks.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While it is intuitively appealing to assume that such variation in learning performance is adaptive ( Johnston 1982;Dukas 1998), few studies have yet been conducted to specifically examine this link under natural conditions. Laboratory studies, using grasshoppers (Dukas & Bernays 2000) and parasitoid wasps (Dukas & Duan 2000), suggest that animals able to form associations between cues (such as colour, odour or location) and rewards perform better than animals prevented from learning. Other laboratory studies, applying artificial selection to the learning ability of fruitflies, provide evidence for potential fitness costs associated with enhanced performance in associative learning (Mery & Kawecki 2003, 2004 or long-term memory (Mery & Kawecki 2005) tasks.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Recent reports suggest that consumers may temporally change diet through learning and phenotypic plasticity (Kause et al 1999;Dukas & Bernays 2000;Egas & Sabelis 2001), which may provide a potential driving force for the introduction of temporal variability in food-web structure (Warren 1989;Winemiller 1990;Eveleigh et al 2007). Given this inherent flexibility in trophic interactions (MacArthur & Pianka 1966;Murdock 1969;Stephens & Krebs 1986), an environmental change that may alter a predator's diet selection behaviour (e.g.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…When faced with such situations, the ability to produce innovative behavior and store it in the repertoire through individual or social learning may have a critical effect on the survival and fitness of individuals (15,(21)(22)(23)(24). Examples include the development of antipredatory responses against novel predators (25), the adoption of new food resources when the traditional ones become scarce (26), or the adjustment of breeding behavior to the prevailing ecological conditions (27).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%