2016
DOI: 10.3758/s13421-016-0669-9
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Learning multiple rules simultaneously: Affixes are more salient than reduplications

Abstract: Language learners encounter numerous opportunities to learn regularities, but need to decide which of these regularities to learn, because some are not productive in their native language. Here, we present an account of rule learning based on perceptual and memory primitives (Endress, Dehaene-Lambertz, & Mehler, Cognition, 105(3), 577–614, 2007; Endress, Nespor, & Mehler, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(8), 348–353, 2009), suggesting that learners preferentially learn regularities that are more salient to the… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 8 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 61 publications
(121 reference statements)
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Interpretation of such data is particularly difficult because learners can process multiple cues simultaneously (e.g., Gervain & Endress, 2017;Endress & Bonatti, 2016;Frank et al, 2009;Gerken et al, 2005;Schonberg et al, 2018;Ter Schure et al, 2014), and might rely on the most reliable (e.g., Frank & Tenenbaum, 2011;Gerken, 2010) or the most salient cue (Endress, 2013;Gervain & Endress, 2017). A case in point is Slone and Johnson's (2018) Experiment 1, where backward TPs are the strongest available cue, and the infants' looking behavior seemed to track the strength of backward TPs in the different test items rather than the frequency of recurring chunks.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Interpretation of such data is particularly difficult because learners can process multiple cues simultaneously (e.g., Gervain & Endress, 2017;Endress & Bonatti, 2016;Frank et al, 2009;Gerken et al, 2005;Schonberg et al, 2018;Ter Schure et al, 2014), and might rely on the most reliable (e.g., Frank & Tenenbaum, 2011;Gerken, 2010) or the most salient cue (Endress, 2013;Gervain & Endress, 2017). A case in point is Slone and Johnson's (2018) Experiment 1, where backward TPs are the strongest available cue, and the infants' looking behavior seemed to track the strength of backward TPs in the different test items rather than the frequency of recurring chunks.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Frank and Tenenbaum (2011) applied this size‐principle‐based model to a variety of infant rule‐learning experiments. However, in addition to the prima facie implausibility of the model, Endress (2013) showed that the models made incorrect predictions (e.g., that a change from human syllables to monkey vocalizations should be less salient than a relatively subtle change from AAB patterns to ABB patterns), assumed that infants can process about 900 triplets per second, made predictions that were subsequently refuted (Gervain & Endress, 2017), assumed that infants have severe perceptual problems in some phases of an experiment and perfect perception in other phases, used model parameters that led their model to contradict the experimental data when the parameters were used in psychologically meaningful ways or wired in the phenomenon they sought to explain (Endress, 2013; see Frank (2013) and Endress (2014) for discussion). It thus seems that an account based on the size principle is unlikely to explain infant rule learning.…”
Section: Are Formal Notions Of Simplicity Empirically Adequate?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Frank and Tenenbaum (2011) applied this size-principlebased model to a variety of infant rule-learning experiments. However, in addition to the prima facie implausibility of the model, Endress (2013) showed that the models made incorrect predictions (e.g., that a change from human syllables to monkey vocalizations should be less salient than a relatively subtle change from AAB patterns to ABB patterns), assumed that infants can process about 900 triplets per second, made predictions that were subsequently refuted (Gervain & Endress, 2017), assumed that infants have severe perceptual problems in some phases of an experiment and perfect perception in other phases, used model parameters that led their model to contradict the experimental data when the parameters were used in psychologically meaningful ways or wired in the phenomenon they sought to explain (Endress, 2013; see Frank (2013) and Endress (2014) for discussion). It thus seems that an account based on the size principle is unlikely to explain infant rule learning.…”
Section: 32mentioning
confidence: 99%