2016
DOI: 10.1111/desc.12397
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Learning the association between a context and a target location in infancy

Abstract: Extracting the statistical regularities present in the environment is a central learning mechanism in infancy. For instance, infants are able to learn the associations between simultaneously or successively presented visual objects (Fiser & Aslin, ; Kirkham, Slemmer & Johnson, ). The present study extends these results by investigating whether infants can learn the association between a target location and the context in which it is presented. With this aim, we used a visual associative learning procedure insp… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

2
13
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(15 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
2
13
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, rather than storing visual contextual information in extensive detail, infants may encode salient features or subsets of features to use as cues (see Brady & Chun, ; Brockmole, Castelhano, & Henderson, for work on local vs. global contextual cueing in adults). In their experiment on target and spatial location associations, Bertels and colleagues () found significant correlations among the distances of certain distracters from the target and the size of infants’ familiarity preference for repeated arrays, which they took as evidence of more local or feature‐based learning. The demands placed on attention and memory systems are likely to impact infants’ performance on search tasks involving more complex or conflicting elements (e.g., Gerhardstein & Rovee‐Collier, ; Scerif, Cornish, Wilding, Driver, & Karmiloff‐Smith, ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For example, rather than storing visual contextual information in extensive detail, infants may encode salient features or subsets of features to use as cues (see Brady & Chun, ; Brockmole, Castelhano, & Henderson, for work on local vs. global contextual cueing in adults). In their experiment on target and spatial location associations, Bertels and colleagues () found significant correlations among the distances of certain distracters from the target and the size of infants’ familiarity preference for repeated arrays, which they took as evidence of more local or feature‐based learning. The demands placed on attention and memory systems are likely to impact infants’ performance on search tasks involving more complex or conflicting elements (e.g., Gerhardstein & Rovee‐Collier, ; Scerif, Cornish, Wilding, Driver, & Karmiloff‐Smith, ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Three‐month‐old infants use spatiotemporal regularities to facilitate attention to upcoming visual events, and by 8 months, they show evidence of orienting to predictive, rather than non‐predictive, spatial cues (Kirkham, Slemmer, Richardson, & Johnson, ; Tummeltshammer & Kirkham, ; Tummeltshammer, Mareschal, & Kirkham, ; Wentworth, Haith, & Hood, ). Recent work has demonstrated that older infants are sensitive to covariation between a target location and the spatial configuration of non‐target elements, looking longer when targets appeared in familiar locations compared to novel locations (Bertels, San Anton, Gebuis, & Destrebecqz, ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For instance, scene-based CC is believed to rely on explicit memory and the global properties of the display (Brockmole et al, 2006;Brockmole & Henderson, 2006a, 2006bBrockmole & Vo, 2010), whereas array-based CC relies on implicit memory (Chun & Jiang, 2003;Colagiuri & Livesey, 2016; but see Vadillo, Konstantinidis, & Shanks, 2016) and the local elements of the display (e.g., Brady & Chun, 2007). Array-based CC is also considered to be a fundamental type of learning, as it was observed in infants (Bertels, San Anton, Gebuis, & Destrebecqz, 2016), nonhuman primates (Goujon & Fagot, 2013), and even birds (Gibson, Leber, & Mehlman, 2015). Thus, it is important to examine whether semantics (in this case, the meaning of the distractors) is a key factor not only in scene-based CC but also in arraybased CC.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some work suggests that the mechanisms that enable memories to guide attention may emerge as early as infancy. Infants as young as 8 months can learn to associate specific contexts with the location of a hidden target (Bertels, San Anton, Gebuis, & Destrebecqz, ) and can use these associations to guide search for targets hidden in simple arrays (Tummeltshammer & Amso, ). These effects may persist into childhood, with children as young as 5 years old demonstrating effects of memories on attention (Dixon et al., ).…”
Section: The Effects Of Salient Visual Events On Attention Orienting mentioning
confidence: 99%