2013
DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.05.004
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Learning to fear obstructed breathing: Comparing interoceptive and exteroceptive cues

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

7
51
0
1

Year Published

2013
2013
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
1

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 44 publications
(59 citation statements)
references
References 25 publications
7
51
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Finally, interoceptive fear conditioning to an interoceptive and exteroceptive conditional stimulus (CS) with a severe respiratory load applied for 30s as the unconditional stimulus resulted in a CS‐load associated larger startle blinks and a smaller decrease in respiratory rate and tidal volume. In comparison, a CS‐picture evoked an increase in tidal volume and self‐reported fear . Therefore, breathing modulation via inspiratory loads is associated with the similar neural and behavioral plasticity that occurs with other exteroceptive stimuli.…”
Section: Resistive Loads—an Experimental Approach To Measure Breathinmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…Finally, interoceptive fear conditioning to an interoceptive and exteroceptive conditional stimulus (CS) with a severe respiratory load applied for 30s as the unconditional stimulus resulted in a CS‐load associated larger startle blinks and a smaller decrease in respiratory rate and tidal volume. In comparison, a CS‐picture evoked an increase in tidal volume and self‐reported fear . Therefore, breathing modulation via inspiratory loads is associated with the similar neural and behavioral plasticity that occurs with other exteroceptive stimuli.…”
Section: Resistive Loads—an Experimental Approach To Measure Breathinmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…Up till now, findings from our lab generally suggest that a fear response is necessary to establish feedforward learning in adult human breathing behavior. Anticipatory fear responses were observed when using breathing loads and occlusions as US (Pappens et al, 2011(Pappens et al, , 2012a(Pappens et al, , 2013. In addition, to these anticipatory fear responses a small change in breathing behavior was observed.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 86%
“…In two experiments, Pappens et al (2013) compared an interoceptive CS (non-aversive resistive load) with an exteroceptive CS (neutral picture) in a fear learning paradigm with an aversive, strong resistive load as the US. They found fear conditioned changes in volume-related breathing parameters.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The limited number of studies using interoceptive stimuli to induce an unpleasant affective state found a potentiation of the startle blink only during anticipation of the aversive interoceptive stimulus (Hubbard et al, 2011;Lang et al, 2011;Melzig et al, 2008;Naliboff et al, 2009;Pappens et al, 2013;Twiss et al, 2009). However, any such potentiation of startle was absent when startles were elicited simultaneously with interoceptive stimulation (Ceunen, Vlaeyen, & Van Diest, 2013;Pappens et al, 2010;.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Also during tonic cold pain and tonic heat pain, which are both interoceptive according to the definition of interoception forwarded by Craig (2002), no startle potentiation has been observed (Deuter et al, 2012;Horn et al, 2012). In contrast, during anxious anticipation of respiratory and other interoceptive sensations, the expected startle potentiation has been found (Hubbard et al, 4 | Startle responding in the context of visceral pain 4 2011; Lang et al, 2011;Melzig et al, 2008;Naliboff et al, 2009;Pappens et al, 2013;Twiss et al, 2009). …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%