2004
DOI: 10.1007/s00265-004-0846-x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Less is more: social learning of predator recognition requires a low demonstrator to observer ratio in Arctic charr (Salvelinus alpinus)

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
42
0

Year Published

2006
2006
2012
2012

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 59 publications
(44 citation statements)
references
References 51 publications
1
42
0
Order By: Relevance
“…These populations are fully maintained by reintroductions from hatchery stocks after hydroelectric schemes destroyed their primary spawning grounds. Initial studies revealed that socially acquired avoidance of predator odours is particularly effective means of training naïve fishes to recognise predators (Vilhunen et al 2005) and these early exposures to predators do lead to improved survival upon subsequent contact with live predators (Vilhunen 2006). …”
Section: Antipredator Behaviourmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These populations are fully maintained by reintroductions from hatchery stocks after hydroelectric schemes destroyed their primary spawning grounds. Initial studies revealed that socially acquired avoidance of predator odours is particularly effective means of training naïve fishes to recognise predators (Vilhunen et al 2005) and these early exposures to predators do lead to improved survival upon subsequent contact with live predators (Vilhunen 2006). …”
Section: Antipredator Behaviourmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This study did not address the recognition of predators by chemical cues, and thus it cannot be ruled out that juveniles recognize piscivores by olfaction. Juvenile fish that are not born with the ability to recognize their predators may acquire or refine that ability by 1 or more of 3 processes: (1) maturation of the sensory or neural system, which does not require exposure to a predator (Munz & McFarland 1973, Coss 1978, Blaxter 1986; (2) individual learning, which may be a dangerous process that involves the exposure of a naive fish to a predator (Csanyi & Doka 1993); and (3) social transmission, by which a naïve fish may learn to recognize predators through exposure to them in company with experienced individuals (Mathis et al 1996, Vilhunen et al 2004. Several recent studies have addressed the problem of social transmission of predator recognition with relation to stock rehabilitation programs and sea ranching of commercial edible fishes (reviewed by Suboski & Templeton 1989, Brown & Laland 2001, but none in connection with reef fishes.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Brown and Laland (2001) suggested that the manipulation of social learning in captivity might improve the viability of hatchery-raised fish in the wild, thus enhancing the population in the wild. An experimental study by Vilhunen, Hirvonen, and Laakkonen (2004) showed the efficacy of social learning as a way to improve antipredator behavior in Saimaa Arctic char (Salvelinus alpinus). In contrast to expectations from theory, the naive fish learned better with a relatively small proportion of experienced fish.…”
Section: Dispersal Migration and Movementmentioning
confidence: 99%