2015
DOI: 10.1177/0142723715574398
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Lexical-phonological interactions in bilingual children

Abstract: This study examined lexical-phonological interactions in the first 50 words of a group of monolingual German-and Spanish-speaking children and bilingual German-Spanish children. The phonological characteristics of the earliest target word forms and output patterns of these children were analyzed to determine whether bilingual children select different target word forms and produce different output forms and templates than monolingual children. Results indicated that the target word forms selected by Spanishspe… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

2
15
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 20 publications
(17 citation statements)
references
References 44 publications
2
15
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In fact, her results showed that speech production proficiency in one language was the best predictor of segmental accuracy in the other language. Other studies of bilingual children learning other language pairs have documented correlations and interaction between phonological systems (Mayr et al, 2015;Kehoe, 2015). Taken together, these findings suggest, in line with the predictions of the Unified Competition Model (MacWhinney, 2005), that speech production abilities in one language may be highly predictive of speech production abilities in the other language, at least when the languages share similar sounds and phonological structures.…”
Section: Relationships Between Phonological Skillssupporting
confidence: 76%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In fact, her results showed that speech production proficiency in one language was the best predictor of segmental accuracy in the other language. Other studies of bilingual children learning other language pairs have documented correlations and interaction between phonological systems (Mayr et al, 2015;Kehoe, 2015). Taken together, these findings suggest, in line with the predictions of the Unified Competition Model (MacWhinney, 2005), that speech production abilities in one language may be highly predictive of speech production abilities in the other language, at least when the languages share similar sounds and phonological structures.…”
Section: Relationships Between Phonological Skillssupporting
confidence: 76%
“…For example, Marchman, Martínez-Sussmann and Dale (2004) and Conboy and Thal (2006) documented strong, positive within-language correlations between lexical and grammatical abilities in bilingual toddlers. Evidence for within-language associations between phonology and other language components has also recently been found (Kehoe, 2011(Kehoe, , 2015Scarpino, 2011). Scarpino, for instance, found that a large portion of the variance in the phonological skills of 199 Spanish-English bilinguals between the ages of 37 and 77 months was predicted by vocabulary scores.…”
Section: Relationships Between Phonological Skillsmentioning
confidence: 77%
“…The influence of the lexicon Studies in monolingual acquisition document a clear relationship between number of words in a child's productive lexicon and phonological ability (Bortolini & Leonard, 2000;Kehoe, Chaplin, Mudry, & Friend, 2015;Paul & Jennings, 1992;Petinou & Okalidou, 2006). To date, the relationship between phonology and the lexicon has not been extensively studied in bilingual children, with some exceptions (Kehoe, 2011(Kehoe, , 2015bScarpino, 2011;Vihman, 2002Vihman, , 2016. Scarpino (2011) examined which factors were the best predictors of phonological production in a large group of Spanish-English children (n = 199), aged 3;0 to 6;4.…”
Section: Factors Accounting For Phonological Performance In Monolingumentioning
confidence: 99%
“…2 Recall that while the present paper focuses on the production domain for the sake of brevity, a complete account of childspecific phonetic pressures would also consider perceptual differences between child and adult speakers. 3 Kehoe (2015) points out that there currently exist no large-scale studies systematically documenting the prevalence of templates and/or lexical selection effects. Until such research is conducted, we are unable to determine whether these phenomena are common (or even universal), or if they characterize only a specific subset of children.…”
Section: Short Biographiesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Kehoe () points out that there currently exist no large‐scale studies systematically documenting the prevalence of templates and/or lexical selection effects. Until such research is conducted, we are unable to determine whether these phenomena are common (or even universal), or if they characterize only a specific subset of children.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%