2013
DOI: 10.1152/jn.00497.2011
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Limb motion dictates how motor learning arises from arbitrary environmental dynamics

Abstract: A key idea in motor learning is that internal models of environmental dynamics are internally represented as functions of spatial variables including position, velocity, and acceleration of body motion. We refer to such a representation as motion dependent. The evidence for a motion-dependent representation is, however, primarily based on examination of the adaptation to motion-dependent dynamic environments. To more rigorously test this idea, we examined the adaptive response to perturbations that cannot be w… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
57
0

Year Published

2013
2013
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 25 publications
(62 citation statements)
references
References 62 publications
5
57
0
Order By: Relevance
“…To accomplish this, we estimated the single-trial adaptation rate induced by the first FF trial of each cycle by placing error-clamp (EC) measurement trials (black bars in Figure 1B) before and after the FF trial in order to determine the change in motor output associated with the FF exposure (blowout in Figure 1B) [14, 15]. We refer to this three-trial sequence as a measurement triplet and used the difference between the lateral force output recorded on the two EC trials in each measurement triplet to measure of the change in feedforward motor output induced by exposure to the intervening FF trial.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To accomplish this, we estimated the single-trial adaptation rate induced by the first FF trial of each cycle by placing error-clamp (EC) measurement trials (black bars in Figure 1B) before and after the FF trial in order to determine the change in motor output associated with the FF exposure (blowout in Figure 1B) [14, 15]. We refer to this three-trial sequence as a measurement triplet and used the difference between the lateral force output recorded on the two EC trials in each measurement triplet to measure of the change in feedforward motor output induced by exposure to the intervening FF trial.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Note that the value of b determines the orientation (clockwise or counterclockwise) and the magnitude of the force field exerted by the manipulandum and has units of N/(m/s). During error-clamp trials, the robot motors were used to constrain movements in a straight line toward the target by counteracting any motion perpendicular to the target direction (Gonzalez Castro et al 2011a;Joiner et al 2011;Joiner and Smith 2008;Scheidt et al 2000;Sing et al 2013;Smith et al 2006;Wagner and Smith 2008). This was achieved by applying a stiff one-dimensional spring (6 kN/m) and damper (150 Ns/m) in the axis perpendicular to the target direction.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…During these error-clamp trials, lateral errors were kept small (Ͻ0.6 mm), so lateral force profiles essentially reflected adaptive compensation of the force-field perturbations. Since full compensation of the force-field perturbation on a particular trial required a lateral force profile proportional to the speed profile on that same trial (and this speed profile varied from one trial to another), we assessed the amount of adaptation on each error-clamp trial by computing a force-field compensation factor found by linear regression of the measured lateral force profile on each error-clamp trial onto the ideal force profile required for full force-field compensation on that trial after subtracting out the baseline force pattern (Gonzalez Castro et al 2011a;Joiner et al 2011;Joiner and Smith 2008;Sing et al 2013;Sing and Smith 2010;Smith et al 2006;Wagner and Smith 2008). Each regression coefficient characterizes the overall amount of force-field compensation in a given trial.…”
Section: Quantifying Adaptation and Transfer With Error-clamp And Formentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Recent studies from Smith and colleagues revealed that adaptation emerges from the actual movement experienced rather than the original motor plan [24], and that even when limb perturbation is not well approximated by spatial variables (e.g. position and velocity), internal models nevertheless represent limb dynamics in a spatial framework [25]. Exploring the role of sensory error signals, Izawa and Shadmehr provide evidence that while both sensory and reward prediction errors drive movement correction, only sensory errors elicit remapping of the forward model of the limb [26].…”
Section: Forward Models For Forelimb Movementmentioning
confidence: 99%