2017
DOI: 10.3758/s13414-017-1390-x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Limits in feature-based attention to multiple colors

Abstract: Attention to a feature enhances the sensory representation of that feature. Although much has been learned about the properties of attentional modulation when attending to a single feature, the effectiveness of attending to multiple features is not well understood. We investigated this question in a series of experiments using a color detection task while varying the number of attended colors in a cueing paradigm. Observers were shown either a single cue, two cues, or no cue (baseline) before detecting a coher… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
20
2

Year Published

2018
2018
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7
3

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 22 publications
(24 citation statements)
references
References 58 publications
2
20
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Here we again found that reward differentially interacted with physical salience (for targets appearing in the coherent vs. random dot field), when preparatory attention was not linked to a particular feature. Our manipulation of attending to both dot fields should weaken the influence of preparatory attention, as supported by previous studies employing multiple attentional templates (Houtkamp & Roelfsema, 2008 ; Stroud, Menneer, Cave, Donnelly, & Rayner, 2011 ; Liu, Becker, & Jigo, 2013 ; Liu & Jigo, 2017 ). However, top-down biasing to a particular feature in this task could be triggered by the speed-up event, which is analogous to a reactive mechanism of cognitive control, where task goals are activated by transient events as needed, rather than sustained in a preparatory state (Braver, 2012 ).…”
Section: Methodssupporting
confidence: 77%
“…Here we again found that reward differentially interacted with physical salience (for targets appearing in the coherent vs. random dot field), when preparatory attention was not linked to a particular feature. Our manipulation of attending to both dot fields should weaken the influence of preparatory attention, as supported by previous studies employing multiple attentional templates (Houtkamp & Roelfsema, 2008 ; Stroud, Menneer, Cave, Donnelly, & Rayner, 2011 ; Liu, Becker, & Jigo, 2013 ; Liu & Jigo, 2017 ). However, top-down biasing to a particular feature in this task could be triggered by the speed-up event, which is analogous to a reactive mechanism of cognitive control, where task goals are activated by transient events as needed, rather than sustained in a preparatory state (Braver, 2012 ).…”
Section: Methodssupporting
confidence: 77%
“…This finding suggests that it was not possible for participants to allocate memory resources using three attentional goals and instead treated all cued items as high priority. Taken together, the results suggest that prioritization of VSTM resources via top-down attention is limited to only two levels, consistent with past work that demonstrating that at least two separate attentional control sets are possible (Adamo et al, 2008(Adamo et al, , 2010Liu, Becker, & Jigo, 2013;Liu & Jigo, 2017).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 90%
“…Second, in line with task switching costs, more generally (Altmann & Gray, 2008;Monsell, 2003), switching the target template can be costly in visual search (Wolfe, Horowitz , Kenner, Hyle, & Vasan, 2004). There are costs associated with switching between two colors (Ort, Fahrenfort, & Olivers, 2017), but these are fairly small (Grubert & Eimer, 2014;Liu & Jigo, 2017) and, as noted above, it could be that two color templates can be active at the same time. The cost is more dramatic in the conjunction search (Wolfe et al, 2004).…”
Section: Figmentioning
confidence: 99%