1986
DOI: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1986.tb01169.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Linear and Curvilinear Relationships Between Temperature, Affect, and Violence: Reply to Cotton

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...

Citation Types

1
10
0

Year Published

1989
1989
2013
2013

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

3
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(11 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
1
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…
Harries and Stadler (1988) observed only a positive linear trend, with no curvilinear relationship, between heat and violence in their 1980-81 Dallas field data. When Cotton (1986) observed similar trends in his data, we noted that in fact the variance in violence increases with temperature (Bell & Fusco, 1986). We report here similar statistically significant heteroscedasticity in the Harries and Stadler data.
…”
supporting
confidence: 77%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…
Harries and Stadler (1988) observed only a positive linear trend, with no curvilinear relationship, between heat and violence in their 1980-81 Dallas field data. When Cotton (1986) observed similar trends in his data, we noted that in fact the variance in violence increases with temperature (Bell & Fusco, 1986). We report here similar statistically significant heteroscedasticity in the Harries and Stadler data.
…”
supporting
confidence: 77%
“…Cotton (1986) provided a good example of field data supporting the linear relationship. We observed, however, that Cotton's data contained statistically significant heteroscedasticity which could actually support the negative affect escape model, as well as higher order trends (Bell & Fusco, 1986). That is, although violence increased with temperature, the variance in the number of violent incidents increased as well.…”
mentioning
confidence: 57%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…According to Baron and Bell's negative affect escape (NAE) model, the discomfort caused by moderately high and low temperatures facilitates aggression, but extreme temperatures arouse competing motives, such as a desire to escape, which conflict with and thereby reduce aggression. Not everybody accepts the NAE model's predictions, as evidenced by vigorous exchanges in the literature (Anderson & DeNeve, 1992;Bell, 1992;Bell & Fusco, 1986). However, we suspect that even its critics will agree that the NAE model satisfied the heuristic function of a theory: The model's counterintuitive predictions generated a considerable amount of research, especially field studies aimed at assessing the external validity of laboratory results.Anderson and Anderson (1998) have proposed a general affective (GA) model of aggression as an alternative to the NAE model.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…in other data, such as studies in which the variance in aggression increases with increases in temperature (potentially allowing for rectilinear and inverted-U relationships in the same data ;Bell & Fusco, 1986, 1989, and that other unmeasured variables such as alcohol consumption or availability of escape can account for inconsistencies in results (Bell, 1992).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%