2016
DOI: 10.3758/s13423-016-1073-y
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Linear grammar as a possible stepping-stone in the evolution of language

Abstract: We suggest that one way to approach the evolution of language is through reverse engineering: asking what components of the language faculty could have been useful in the absence of the full complement of components. We explore the possibilities offered by linear grammar, a form of language that lacks syntax and morphology altogether, and that structures its utterances through a direct mapping between semantics and phonology. A language with a linear grammar would have no syntactic categories or syntactic phra… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
25
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 55 publications
(27 citation statements)
references
References 39 publications
1
25
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The core findings, as taught in introductory courses in linguistics or psychology of language, have been reviewed in detail in many places (e.g., Crystal, 2003;Yule, 2006). Although the correct theoretical and philosophical framework for understanding this picture remains a topic of discussion (see, e.g., the articles by Arbib, 2016;Chomsky, 2016;Jackendoff & Wittenberg, 2016;ScottPhillips, 2016), it all concerns modern humans and thus defines the Bend target^of any evolutionary model, rather than the steps required to get to this point. It is only rather recently that detailed theoretical models of modern language have been used to fuel hypotheses about how language evolved (e.g., Berwick & Chomsky, 2016;Givón, 2002;Hurford, 2011;Jackendoff, 2002Jackendoff, , 2010Scott-Phillips, 2014), though earlier efforts include Bickerton (1990) and Pinker and Bloom (1990).…”
Section: Empirical Data Relevant To Testing Models Of Language Evolutionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The core findings, as taught in introductory courses in linguistics or psychology of language, have been reviewed in detail in many places (e.g., Crystal, 2003;Yule, 2006). Although the correct theoretical and philosophical framework for understanding this picture remains a topic of discussion (see, e.g., the articles by Arbib, 2016;Chomsky, 2016;Jackendoff & Wittenberg, 2016;ScottPhillips, 2016), it all concerns modern humans and thus defines the Bend target^of any evolutionary model, rather than the steps required to get to this point. It is only rather recently that detailed theoretical models of modern language have been used to fuel hypotheses about how language evolved (e.g., Berwick & Chomsky, 2016;Givón, 2002;Hurford, 2011;Jackendoff, 2002Jackendoff, , 2010Scott-Phillips, 2014), though earlier efforts include Bickerton (1990) and Pinker and Bloom (1990).…”
Section: Empirical Data Relevant To Testing Models Of Language Evolutionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…B. Partee, ter Meulen, & Wall, 1990), semantic composition is considered to be a special case of syntactic composition, and syntax determines the meaning of a phrase or a clause. However, according to an alternative perspective, semantic composition can proceed (partially or fully) independently from syntactic structure building (e.g., Jackendoff & Jackendoff, 2002;Jackendoff, 2007;Culicover & Jackendoff, 2006;Culicover, Jackendoff, Jackendoff, et al, 2005;Kuperberg, 2007;Jackendoff, 2010;Jackendoff & Wittenberg, 2017). Baggio (2018) refers to this idea as "autonomous semantics".…”
Section: The Relationship Between Lexico-semantic and Syntactic Procementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Discussion of the increasing value of number of arguments (so-called one word, two word, n-word utterances) has figured prominently for many decades in studies of first language acquisition, and more recently in discussions of language evolution: from n = 0 (silence is no doubt an also an option for communication), through n = 1 for single word utterances, n = 2 for binary, and n > 3 for the general case. Jackendoff and Wittenberg (2016) suggest that the move from sequences of 2 to sequences of 3 was a qualitative leap in language evolution. Qualitative leaps are also represented by each move beyond sequences of 3, followed by unidirectional recursion and then by the centreembedding generalisation.…”
Section: Linear Sequences Iteration and Regular Grammarsmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…The default procedural plausibility criteria of linear time and finite working memory are at the very least a useful starting point and a potentially falsifiable heuristic benchmark, perhaps even a necessary theoretical core. As complexity increases during the ontogeny and phylogeny of gestural, spoken and written communication, increasingly complex linear patterns appear, from the simplest sound sequences through the basic clausal patterns proposed by Wittenberg and Jackendoff (2014) and Jackendoff and Wittenberg (2016) to the centre-embedding generalisation with its processing problems. We claim that the centre-embedding emerges as a generalisation of non-centre-embedding recursion on the different time scales of spontaneous speech improvisation, of language acquisition, of the history of language change and of language evolution, a claim which is speculative but not implausible, and far simpler and potentially easier to falsify than claims about the triggering of recursion by genetic mutation (though not excluding this possibility).…”
Section: Flat Grammarsmentioning
confidence: 99%