2012
DOI: 10.1002/acp.2818
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Listen Out! Behavioural and Subjective Responses to Verbal Warnings

Abstract: Both the behavioural and subjective impacts of single-word spoken warnings were examined. Behaviourally, the effect of infrequently occurring warnings was studied through their disruptive impact on a visually presented serial recall task. In separate experiments, ratings of the same words were elicited. Experiment 1 showed a localized effect of the warnings (on the item immediately following the warning), with the urgently intoned warning having a greater disruptive effect than its valence (emotional content).… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

2
15
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

2
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 21 publications
(17 citation statements)
references
References 35 publications
2
15
0
Order By: Relevance
“…While the degree to which an event occurs unexpectedly seems to play an important role [8,9,24] there are other determinants as well that affect the capability of a stimulus to attract attention such as the valence of a distractor word [25,26] or the urgency with which a verbal warning is intoned. [27] Further, the recovery from attentional capture seems to depend partly on individual differences in working memory capacity, too: Participants with greater cognitive control abilities showed an increased habituation rate. [28] From an interference-by-process perspective [16,29] the disruptive effect of a distractor increases with the degree to which it competes for action.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…While the degree to which an event occurs unexpectedly seems to play an important role [8,9,24] there are other determinants as well that affect the capability of a stimulus to attract attention such as the valence of a distractor word [25,26] or the urgency with which a verbal warning is intoned. [27] Further, the recovery from attentional capture seems to depend partly on individual differences in working memory capacity, too: Participants with greater cognitive control abilities showed an increased habituation rate. [28] From an interference-by-process perspective [16,29] the disruptive effect of a distractor increases with the degree to which it competes for action.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…In fact, recent evidence suggests that the capture of attention by urgently spoken word distracters can be especially detrimental when occurring while participants are encoding information in memory for later recall (Ljungberg, Parmentier, Hughes, Macken, & Jones, 2012). This evidence lends support to the contention that the smaller lengthening of reaction times for urgently spoken words than for nonurgently spoken words (all relative to the standard condition) must involve a response speeding for the former.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In a typical experiment, participants must recall lists of visually presented items (usually digits) in the order of their presentation. When task-irrelevant, to-be-ignored background sound is played, serial recall performance is reduced compared to a quiet control condition (Colle & Welsh, 1976; see also Elliott, 2002; Ljungberg, Parmentier, Hughes, Macken & Jones, 2012; Marsh, Hughes & Jones, 2009; Röer, Bell & Buchner, 2013; Salamé & Baddeley, 1982). This reduction is largely independent of the auditory distractors’ sound level (Ellermeier & Hellbrück, 1998), and of whether the distractors are played during item encoding or maintenance (Jones, Madden & Miles, 1992; Röer, Bell & Buchner, 2014c).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%