2005
DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00357.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Long‐term memory is the representational basis for semantic verbal short‐term memory

Abstract: The present study supports activation models of verbal short-term memory that include a semantic contribution to the retention process. Event-related brain potentials were used to probe the level of activation of semantic representations of a series of words in a delay interval following their presentation. The levels of activation were compared in two tasks: (1) a short-term memory task that involved a semantic judgment in the recall phase following the delay interval, and (2) a nonmemory control task. The le… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
39
1

Year Published

2007
2007
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 32 publications
(41 citation statements)
references
References 59 publications
1
39
1
Order By: Relevance
“…That is, delay-period activity from every ROI were reliably classified as matching the stimulus category of the trial. This result indicates that the classifier successfully differentiated visuospatial from phonological (Baddeley, 1986) from semantic (Haarmann & Usher, 2001; Martin, Wu, Freedman, Jackson, & Lesch, 2003; Shivde & Thompson-Schill, 2004; Cameron, Haarmann, Grafman, & Ruchkin, 2005) STM, and all three from the resting state activity recorded during the ITI. Prediction accuracy for each category in all ROIs was significantly above chance based on independent-sample t-tests across participants.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 80%
“…That is, delay-period activity from every ROI were reliably classified as matching the stimulus category of the trial. This result indicates that the classifier successfully differentiated visuospatial from phonological (Baddeley, 1986) from semantic (Haarmann & Usher, 2001; Martin, Wu, Freedman, Jackson, & Lesch, 2003; Shivde & Thompson-Schill, 2004; Cameron, Haarmann, Grafman, & Ruchkin, 2005) STM, and all three from the resting state activity recorded during the ITI. Prediction accuracy for each category in all ROIs was significantly above chance based on independent-sample t-tests across participants.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 80%
“…However, substantial evidence from behavioral (Rose, Buchsbaum, & Craik, 2014;Shivde & Anderson, 2011), neuropsychological (Barde, Schwartz, Chrysikou, & Thompson-Schill, 2010;Hamilton & Martin, 2007;Martin, 2005), EEG (Cameron, Haarmann, Grafman, & Ruchkin, 2005;Ruchkin, Grafman, Cameron, & Berndt, 2003), and fMRI (Fiebach, Friederici, Smith, & Swinney, 2007;Shivde & ThompsonSchill, 2004) studies suggest that deeper, semantic codes and associated representational cortical areas do support WM in many situations. For instance, deeper (semantic) LOP at encoding benefits WM when active maintenance processes are disrupted (Rose et al, 2014).…”
Section: Semantic Wmmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…In addition, Ruchkin et al (1999Ruchkin et al ( , 2003 report event-related potential (ERP) effects of lexical status, word frequency, and word concreteness during the retention period of a verbal WM task. Furthermore, Cameron, Haarmann, Grafman, and Ruchkin (2005) report semantic priming effects on the N400 ERP component for incidental probes related to the words of the memory list, during the retention period of a verbal WM task. Finding a reduction in an ERP component sensitive to semantic context effects suggests that the processing of the incidental probe is facilitated by the sustained activation of the semantic representations of the memory set, lending support to the notion of sustained activation of conceptual-semantic representations during the short-term maintenance of words in WM.…”
Section: Neural Mechanisms Of Semantic Working Memorymentioning
confidence: 97%