1989
DOI: 10.3758/bf03202635
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Looking through phonological shape to lexical meaning: The bottleneck of non-native sign language processing

Abstract: In two studies, we find that native and non-native acquisition show different effects on sign language processing. Subjects were all born deaf and used sign language for interpersonal communication, but first acquired it at ages ranging from birth to 18. In the first study, deaf signers shadowed (simultaneously watched and reproduced) sign language narratives given in two dialects, American Sign Language (ASL) and Pidgin Sign English (PSE), in both good and poor viewing conditions. In the second study, deaf si… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

11
108
1

Year Published

2000
2000
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 147 publications
(120 citation statements)
references
References 34 publications
11
108
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Thus, they may actually be best considered as late functional L1-sign learners, a situation that poses unique difficulties in language learning and perception that lead them to attend more to the fine-grained phonetic properties of signs, including phonologically irrelevant details, than do DE signers or, indeed, either of the native-English hearing groups. This picture is consistent with prior reports that DL signers make significantly more phonological errors in sign recall than do DE signers, who make more semantic errors (Mayberry & Fischer, 1989). In fact, there is a tendency for phonological errors to increase with the AoA of ASL (Mayberry & Eichen, 1991).…”
Section: Axb Discriminationsupporting
confidence: 82%
“…Thus, they may actually be best considered as late functional L1-sign learners, a situation that poses unique difficulties in language learning and perception that lead them to attend more to the fine-grained phonetic properties of signs, including phonologically irrelevant details, than do DE signers or, indeed, either of the native-English hearing groups. This picture is consistent with prior reports that DL signers make significantly more phonological errors in sign recall than do DE signers, who make more semantic errors (Mayberry & Fischer, 1989). In fact, there is a tendency for phonological errors to increase with the AoA of ASL (Mayberry & Eichen, 1991).…”
Section: Axb Discriminationsupporting
confidence: 82%
“…Because 90-95% of Deaf children are born to hearing parents who do not sign, native-like language acquisition which results from early and consistent exposure to a language is not the norm. Previous work has demonstrated that age of exposure to sign language results in subtle but measurable differences in processing Corina & Hildebrandt, 2002;Mayberry & Fischer, 1989;Newman et al, 2002;Newport, 1990). During sentence recall and shadowing, for example, late learners of ASL produce a disproportionate number of phonological substitutions (i.e., signs that are phonologically similar to the target signs but differ in meaning) relative to native signers (Mayberry, Lock, & Kazmi, 2002).…”
Section: Age Of Acquisitionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The significance of many of these factors has been well documented in the literature, specifically age of onset of deafness (Allen & Osborn, 1984;Marschark & Spencer, 2003), age and level of sign language exposure (Mayberry, 1993(Mayberry, , 1994Mayberry & Eichen, 1991;Mayberry & Fischer, 1989), parental hearing status (Erting, 1994;Mitchell & Karchmer, 2005;Morford & Mayberry, 2000;Moores, 2001;Padden & Humphries, 1988), and quality of language input (Singleton & Newport, 2004). Deaf children's language experiences vary considerably, ranging from deaf parents, who provide early access to language through sign to hearing parents who communicate exclusively through speech.…”
Section: Influencing Factors On Deaf Children's Lexical Acquisitionmentioning
confidence: 99%