2011
DOI: 10.1038/npp.2010.239
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Loss of Alternative Non-Drug Reinforcement Induces Relapse of Cocaine-Seeking in Rats: Role of Dopamine D1 Receptors

Abstract: Animal models of relapse to drug seeking have focused primarily on relapse induced by exposure to drugs, drug-associated cues or contexts, and foot-shock stress. However, relapse in human drug abusers is often precipitated by loss of alternative non-drug reinforcement. The present experiment used a novel “resurgence” paradigm to examine relapse to cocaine seeking of rats as a result of loss of an alternative source of non-drug reinforcement. Rats were first trained to press a lever for intravenous infusions of… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

7
63
0

Year Published

2013
2013
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 59 publications
(70 citation statements)
references
References 35 publications
7
63
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This simulation in Fig. 1 is supported by evidence from two studies, Quick et al (2011) and Wacker et al (2011). Quick et al investigated resurgence of cocaine seeking in rats following the removal of alternative food reinforcement for nose pokes during extinction.…”
mentioning
confidence: 65%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This simulation in Fig. 1 is supported by evidence from two studies, Quick et al (2011) and Wacker et al (2011). Quick et al investigated resurgence of cocaine seeking in rats following the removal of alternative food reinforcement for nose pokes during extinction.…”
mentioning
confidence: 65%
“…Many popular behavioral treatments involve alternative reinforcement, such as contingency management for substance abuse (e.g., Higgins et al, 2010; Silverman et al, 2007) and differential reinforcement of alternative behavior (DRA) in individuals with intellectual or developmental disabilities (Petscher, Rey, & Bailey, 2009). Although treatments that use alternative reinforcement are often effective at reducing problem behavior during treatment, the risk of relapse when alternative reinforcement is reduced or removed has led to a recent revival in basic (Lieving & Lattal, 2003; Quick, Pyszczynski, Colston, & Shahan, 2011; Winterbauer & Bouton, 2010) and applied (Volkert, Lerman, Call,& Trosclaire-Lasserre, 2009) research on resurgence (see Lattal & St. Peter Pipkin, 2009, for a review).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A potential advantage of the resurgence preparations used by Podlesnik et al (2006) and Quick et al (2011) and the resurgence-like experimental arrangement used in the present experiments is that they may be useful in modeling the events surrounding relapse experienced by some drug users. For instance, removal of rewarding events, such as unemployment, can cause increased drug intake and trigger relapse (Temple et al, 1991;Gallo et al, 2001;Falba et al, 2005).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Our laboratory has also shown that loss of an alternative nondrug source of reinforcement can produce increases in extinguished alcohol-seeking (Podlesnik et al, 2006) or cocaine-seeking (Quick et al, 2011) of rats in a resurgence paradigm (see Marchant et al, 2013, for a review). Within the resurgence procedure, a response (R1) is first maintained through contingent drug deliveries.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…However, strong preference to foods are a complex interaction of many factors, difficult to study even in highly controlled research settings with rodents (CORWIN et al, 2011). To answer this question definitively will likely require research in which rodents self administer purified swainsonine, similar to strong preference studies with compounds of abuse in which rats are first trained to press a lever for intravenous infusions (e.g., QUICK et al, 2011).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%