1991
DOI: 10.1080/10862969109547725
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Low-Ses Children's Success and Failure at Early Literacy Learning in Skills-Based Classrooms

Abstract: This study examined low-SES, urban children's ways of interpreting traditional skills-based literacy instruction in kindergarten and first grade. Thirty-five randomly selected children from three inner-city schools were tested for entering and end-of-first-grade knowledge of six domains of written language. Their scores on two standardized achievement tests were also collected. Twelve children were randomly selected from this sample for close observation over 2 years in their classrooms. Qualitative and quanti… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
62
0
4

Year Published

1991
1991
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
10

Relationship

2
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 93 publications
(67 citation statements)
references
References 9 publications
1
62
0
4
Order By: Relevance
“…Avant l'intervention, peu de garderies disposaient de coins de lecture; 93% en avaient un après l'intervention. Les enfants dont les éducatrices avaient reçu un entraînement manifestèrent par ailleurs un niveau de développe-ment plus élevé de la conscience de l'écrit (Clay, 1979), de la compétence narrative (Purcell-Gates, 1991;Purcell-Gates et Dahl, 1991), des concepts de l'écrit (PurcellGates, 1996), du nom des lettres (Clay, 1979) que les enfants du groupe contrôle. Un suivi en maternelle a confirmé que les enfants du groupe expérimental avaient maintenu leur avance pour ce qui concerne le nom des lettres (Clay, 1979), la conscience phonologique mesurée par la reconnaissance de rimes et d'allitérations (Maclean, Bryant et Bradley, 1987) et les concepts de l'écrit (Purcell-Gates, 1996).…”
Section: La Motivation Pour La Lectureunclassified
“…Avant l'intervention, peu de garderies disposaient de coins de lecture; 93% en avaient un après l'intervention. Les enfants dont les éducatrices avaient reçu un entraînement manifestèrent par ailleurs un niveau de développe-ment plus élevé de la conscience de l'écrit (Clay, 1979), de la compétence narrative (Purcell-Gates, 1991;Purcell-Gates et Dahl, 1991), des concepts de l'écrit (PurcellGates, 1996), du nom des lettres (Clay, 1979) que les enfants du groupe contrôle. Un suivi en maternelle a confirmé que les enfants du groupe expérimental avaient maintenu leur avance pour ce qui concerne le nom des lettres (Clay, 1979), la conscience phonologique mesurée par la reconnaissance de rimes et d'allitérations (Maclean, Bryant et Bradley, 1987) et les concepts de l'écrit (Purcell-Gates, 1996).…”
Section: La Motivation Pour La Lectureunclassified
“…Current research indicates that when young readers' concepts are not aligned with the focus of instruction, undesirable results ensue. Without sound understanding of reading as a meaning-making process, that is, without having the big picture, children cannot benefit from being taught about the abstract or graphophonic innerworkings of print (Dyson, 1982;Purcell-Gates & Dahl, 1991).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Yet Tom's difficulty with learning to read cannot be laid at the door of an impoverished home background. Nor, since Kathrin was also his first-grade teacher, did he seem to have suffered from a mismatch between the holistic and meaningful view of literacy in his home and some sort of skills-based, meaning-free reading instruction at school, as has been postulated to cause some cases of reading difficulty in children from highly literate homes (e.g., Michel, 1994;Purcell-Gates & Dahl, 1991). In education, we often conclude that if the explanation for student underachievement lies not in the environment, it must lie within the student himself; indeed, this is sometimes the preferred explanation.…”
Section: Must Delayed Readers Necessarily Be "At-risk" For Reading Famentioning
confidence: 99%