2022
DOI: 10.1038/s41598-022-06503-1
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Lower multisensory temporal acuity in individuals with high schizotypal traits: a web-based study

Abstract: Natural events are often multisensory, requiring the brain to combine information from the same spatial location and timing, across different senses. The importance of temporal coincidence has led to the introduction of the temporal binding window (TBW) construct, defined as the time range within which multisensory inputs are highly likely to be perceptually bound into a single entity. Anomalies in TBWs have been linked to confused perceptual experiences and inaccurate filtering of sensory inputs coming from d… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2022
2022
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 104 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…These findings regarding the important role that decision criteria play stand in contrast to the widespread interpretation of simultaneity-function width as an unambiguous measure of the precision of multisensory integration (e.g., Chen et al, 2017; Foucher et al, 2007; Habets et al, 2017; Hillock et al, 2011; Lee & Noppeney, 2011; Marsicano et al, 2022; Navarra & Fernández-Prieto, 2020; Noel et al, 2017; Scarpina et al, 2016; Stevenson et al, 2014; Zampini et al, 2005). We have already indicated how our results show that simultaneity-function width in uninstructed baseline conditions is not a measure of a hard sensory threshold, if indeed one exists.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 73%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…These findings regarding the important role that decision criteria play stand in contrast to the widespread interpretation of simultaneity-function width as an unambiguous measure of the precision of multisensory integration (e.g., Chen et al, 2017; Foucher et al, 2007; Habets et al, 2017; Hillock et al, 2011; Lee & Noppeney, 2011; Marsicano et al, 2022; Navarra & Fernández-Prieto, 2020; Noel et al, 2017; Scarpina et al, 2016; Stevenson et al, 2014; Zampini et al, 2005). We have already indicated how our results show that simultaneity-function width in uninstructed baseline conditions is not a measure of a hard sensory threshold, if indeed one exists.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 73%
“…By way of example, in recent years it has become fashionable to measure “temporal binding windows” using just the SJ task, and interpret differences between groups or conditions as indicative of differences in the temporal sensitivity of integration processes (e.g., Chen et al, 2017; Foucher et al, 2007; Habets et al, 2017; Hillock et al, 2011; Lee & Noppeney, 2011; Marsicano et al, 2022; Navarra & Fernández-Prieto, 2020; Noel et al, 2017; Scarpina et al, 2016; Stevenson et al, 2014; Zampini et al, 2005). While of considerable interest, we believe that much of this work does not include sufficiently explicit caveats about the processes that might generate the window of simultaneity, potentially misrepresenting the underlying cause(s) of differences between conditions/groups.…”
Section: Recent Treatments Of Sj Datamentioning
confidence: 99%
“…[32][33][34][35][36] Studies suggest that fear conditioning, changes in the immune system and metabolic changes are also responsible for the development of these neurological disorders. [37][38][39][40] Therefore, these disorders need to be detected in a timely manner using various neuroimaging modalities [41][42][43][44] and should be treated using available strategies such as behavioral therapy, family therapy or with anti-psychotic medications. 45,46 Indeed, NRIs are widely applied in the treatment of mental disorders.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, SJ tasks use an index calculated based on the temporal distribution of the simultaneous response rate (usually a bell‐shaped Gaussian curve). Indexes include: (1) the interval between the two SOA values corresponding to a 75% simultaneous response rate (Marsicano et al., 2022 ; Venskus et al., 2021 ; Zerr et al., 2019 ); (2) the interval between the SOA value corresponding to the point of subjective simultaneity and the SOA value corresponding to a 75% simultaneous response rate (just noticeable difference, JND) (Christie et al., 2019 ; Li et al., 2021 ); (3) half of the interval between the two SOA values corresponding to a 50% simultaneous response rate ( δ ) (Chen et al., 2018 , 2021 ); (4) the standard deviation of the distribution (SD or σ ) (Yarrow et al., 2016 ; Zampini et al., 2005 ); (5) the interval between the two SOA values corresponding to 50% of the maximum rate (full width at half height, FWHH) (Alm & Behne, 2014 ; Roseboom & Arnold, 2011 ; Roseboom et al., 2009 ); and (6) half of the interval between the two SOA values corresponding to 50% of the maximum rate (half width at half height, HWHH) (Fujisaki & Nishida, 2009 ). The larger these values, the larger the TBW, resulting in a lower temporal resolution of synchrony perception.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%