2015
DOI: 10.3758/s13428-014-0560-1
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

LSE-Sign: A lexical database for Spanish Sign Language

Abstract: The LSE-Sign database is a free online tool for selecting Spanish Sign Language stimulus materials to be used in experiments. It contains 2,400 individual signs taken from a recent standardized LSE dictionary, and a further 2,700 related nonsigns. Each entry is coded for a wide range of grammatical, phonological, and articulatory information, including handshape, location, movement, and non-manual elements. The database is accessible via a graphically based search facility which is highly flexible both in term… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
17
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 33 publications
(17 citation statements)
references
References 34 publications
0
17
0
Order By: Relevance
“…One possibility is that some of the non-signers treated the non-sign repetition as a visuospatial receptive task, increasing the involvement of the right hemisphere, as has been shown in previous fTCD studies of visuo-spatial processing (e.g., Payne et al, 2015; Rosch, Bishop, & Badcock, 2012; Whitehouse, Badcock, Groen, & Bishop, 2009). Another possible explanation for the variability within the non-signing group is that, although the non-signs used in the current study had been rated as non-iconic (Gutierrez-Sigut, Costello, et al, 2015) and the instructions emphasized that they had no meaning, it is possible that some participants sought meaning in the signs, and therefore increased the involvement of the left hemisphere. Finally, it is possible that the fTCD signal was weaker in some of the non-signers because they were performing a repetition task rather than the generation task performed by signers.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 96%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…One possibility is that some of the non-signers treated the non-sign repetition as a visuospatial receptive task, increasing the involvement of the right hemisphere, as has been shown in previous fTCD studies of visuo-spatial processing (e.g., Payne et al, 2015; Rosch, Bishop, & Badcock, 2012; Whitehouse, Badcock, Groen, & Bishop, 2009). Another possible explanation for the variability within the non-signing group is that, although the non-signs used in the current study had been rated as non-iconic (Gutierrez-Sigut, Costello, et al, 2015) and the instructions emphasized that they had no meaning, it is possible that some participants sought meaning in the signs, and therefore increased the involvement of the left hemisphere. Finally, it is possible that the fTCD signal was weaker in some of the non-signers because they were performing a repetition task rather than the generation task performed by signers.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…One hundred non-sign video clips were chosen from LSE-Sign: a lexical database for Spanish Sign Language (Gutierrez-Sigut, Costello, Baus, & Carreiras, 2015). Video clips for non-signs in this database were recorded by varying one phonological parameter from an existing Spanish sign language sign.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Phonological information for hand orientation and movement were omitted. Phonological features such as handshape and location were coded based on the Prosodic Model (Brentari 1998) and similar types of coding can be found in larger corpora of sign languages (e.g., Gutierrez-Sigut et al 2015).…”
Section: Stimulimentioning
confidence: 99%