2002
DOI: 10.1117/12.454924
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

<title>Correct 3-D but illusory 2-D percepts linked to binocularly unpaired regions</title>

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

3
18
0

Year Published

2003
2003
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

4
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(21 citation statements)
references
References 9 publications
3
18
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Our initial interest in the illusion was the “novelty” of a stimulus appearing displaced toward the occluder: a result that contrasts recent discussions of Leonardo's constraint in which apparent displacement is away from the occluder (e.g., Ohtsuka & Ono, 2002; Ono et al., 2003). However, van Ee et al.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 82%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Our initial interest in the illusion was the “novelty” of a stimulus appearing displaced toward the occluder: a result that contrasts recent discussions of Leonardo's constraint in which apparent displacement is away from the occluder (e.g., Ohtsuka & Ono, 2002; Ono et al., 2003). However, van Ee et al.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 82%
“…See Figure 2b,c for examples of no displacement and displacement away from the occluder, respectively.) The illusory displacements of monocularly seen stimuli that have been reported previously, however, are always away from an occluder (e.g., van Ee, Banks, & Backus, 1999;Ohtsuka & Ono, 2002;Ono, Lillakas, Grove, & Suzuki, 2003). For instance, van Ee et al (1999) reported that the monocularly seen segment of a vertical line (a line monocularly occluded in the center by a rectangle), appeared displaced away from the occluder.…”
mentioning
confidence: 93%
“…Specifically, given the displacements reported in the present study when an occluder is fixated, one would expect that (a) the square, which is occluded in its midportion, would appear rectangular because of the outward displacement of its two vertical sides (i.e., horizontally elongated) and (b) the two oblique lines would not appear collinear because of the outward displacements of their abutted ends. Studies (Ohtsuka & Ono, 2002;Ono et al, 1998;van Ee & Erkelens, 2000) that measured both shape perception and visual direction, however, show that these two predicted distortions do not occur in three-dimensional perception. Taken together, these results suggest that visual-direction information and shape and alignment information are processed by two separate mechanisms and that the shape and alignment mechanism corrects for the expected distortions illustrated in Figure 6B.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is similar to Gregory's (1963) hypothesis about the inappropriate constancy scaling as an explanation for the Mueller-Lyer illusion (see Day & Knuth, 1982;Robinson, 1972). Moreover, the ideas presented here address theoretical puzzles, namely, (a) why shape perception remains veridical in three-dimensional perceptual space despite visual direction being nonveridical (Ohtsuka & Ono, 2002;van Ee & Erkelens, 2000); (b) why in three-dimensional perceptual space, a square behind an occluder is seen as a square (Ohtsuka, 1995a;Ono et al, 1998) and an oblique line behind an occluder is seen aligned (Drobnis & Lawson, 1976;Gyoba, 1978;Liu & Kennedy, 1995;Ohtsuka, 1995b); and (c) why the end of an oblique line that abuts an occluder in a drawing appears more misaligned than does its far end (Wenderoth, 1980). To propose that the underlying mechanism for visual direction examined in this study is different from that for shape and alignment is plausible but ad hoc.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation