1978
DOI: 10.1364/josa.68.001767
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Luminous efficiency functions determined by successive brightness matching

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
16
0

Year Published

1983
1983
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 26 publications
(17 citation statements)
references
References 13 publications
1
16
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The observer attempted to ignore the colour in making the luminance judgements, which were both highly consistent between the two observers and well predicted from the motion data. Ikeda & Shimozono (1978) have shown that observers can make accurate heterochromatic luminance settings with square-wave flicker as low as 2 Hz, even though chromatic flicker is present.…”
Section: Relative Temporal Phase Response Of S and L Cones In Motion mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The observer attempted to ignore the colour in making the luminance judgements, which were both highly consistent between the two observers and well predicted from the motion data. Ikeda & Shimozono (1978) have shown that observers can make accurate heterochromatic luminance settings with square-wave flicker as low as 2 Hz, even though chromatic flicker is present.…”
Section: Relative Temporal Phase Response Of S and L Cones In Motion mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These measures have been described thoroughly elsewhere; Pages successive heterochromatic brightness matching has been described by Ikeda and Shimozono (1978) and the other techniques by Wyszecki and Stiles (1982). The resulting relative sensitivity functions derived from these methods fall generally into two categories.…”
Section: F = S 683 Y(kw(l)mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Therefore I decided to investigate luminous efficiency functions at high intensities using two psychophysical techniques: One that is representative of the visual performance based on achromatic system alone, namely flicker photometry and one that would tap into both the achromatic and chromatic systems, successive heterochromatic brightness matching. Successive heterochromatic brightness matching is felt to produce similar results to the traditional bipartite heterochromatic brightness matching and has the advantage of using the same apparatus as flicker photometry (Ikeda and Shimozono, 1978). The major goal was to compare results of the 2 methods directly, using the same optical system and subjects in order to explore how luminance efficiency functions change with increasing intensity so that we can better predict the visual perception of spectrally narrow and bright light sources.…”
Section: F = S 683 Y(kw(l)mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the HFP task, subjects viewed a stimulus flickering between red and green (both in the absence and in the presence of surrounding black/white stimuli) and adjusted the relative luminance of the two colors until the percept of flicker was least salient. [7][8][9] In the MDB task, subjects viewed red and green stimuli placed in spatial juxtaposition (both in the absence and in the presence of surrounding black/white stimuli) and adjusted the relative luminance of the two colors until the border between them was least salient. 10 Note that unlike for HBM, for HFP and MDB the subject's task does not involve comparing any property of the red versus green stimulus per se.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%