2008
DOI: 10.1037/a0013012
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Making sense of word senses: The comprehension of polysemy depends on sense overlap.

Abstract: Studies of polysemy are few in number and are contradictory. Some have found differences between polysemy and homonymy (L. Frazier & K. Rayner, 1990), and others have found similarities (D. K. Klein & G. Murphy, 2001). The authors investigated this issue using the methods of D. K. Klein and G. Murphy (2001), in whose study participants judged whether ambiguous words embedded in word pairs (e.g., tasty chicken) made sense as a function of a cooperating, conflicting, or neutral context. The ambiguous words were … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

9
213
1

Year Published

2012
2012
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 118 publications
(223 citation statements)
references
References 49 publications
9
213
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The different senses of, e.g., 'school', do not look like specifications of a more general, abstract, meaning in this same way (it could be held that school-the-building specifies the meaning of 'school', but it is difficult to say that what it specifies is a general, abstract, meaning). That is, whereas the regular polysemy that we find in some nouns cannot be accounted for in terms of common cores (Klepousniotou et al, 2008), or properly underspecific (vs. overspecific) representations (Frisson, 2009), the different senses of verbs do seem to share some features, and so to have a common core. I have argued elsewhere (Vicente, 2017) that this is due to the fact that the polysemy that verbs typically display is metaphor-based, whereas the regular polysemy patterns that nouns such as 'school' display is metonymy-based.…”
Section: Not All Meanings Are Rich? the Case Of Verbsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…The different senses of, e.g., 'school', do not look like specifications of a more general, abstract, meaning in this same way (it could be held that school-the-building specifies the meaning of 'school', but it is difficult to say that what it specifies is a general, abstract, meaning). That is, whereas the regular polysemy that we find in some nouns cannot be accounted for in terms of common cores (Klepousniotou et al, 2008), or properly underspecific (vs. overspecific) representations (Frisson, 2009), the different senses of verbs do seem to share some features, and so to have a common core. I have argued elsewhere (Vicente, 2017) that this is due to the fact that the polysemy that verbs typically display is metaphor-based, whereas the regular polysemy patterns that nouns such as 'school' display is metonymy-based.…”
Section: Not All Meanings Are Rich? the Case Of Verbsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These are all productive and regular patterns, and most of them can be found in many languages of the world (Srinivasan and Rabagliati, 2015). One typical way of dealing with these cases is by postulating different representations stored either in two separate lexical entries or within a single lexical entry (sense-enumeration lexicons) (Foraker and Murphy, 2012); another is to postulate a core common meaning (Klepousniotou, et al, 2008, Frisson, 2009; and yet another is to posit richer conceptual representations (Pustejovsky, 1995).…”
Section: Regular Polysemymentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…The difference between balanced and unbalanced ambiguous words is most pronounced in the case of homonyms-words for which a single written and spoken form is associated with multiple unrelated interpretations, and for which there is general agreement that the semantic overlap between the interpretations is minimal (e.g., <dog>/<tree> BARK; Armstrong & Plaut, 2008;Frazier & Rayner, 1990;Hino, Kusunose, & Lupker, 2010;Hino et al, 2006;Klein & Murphy, 2001Klepousniotou et al, 2008;Rodd et al, 2002;Rodd, Gaskell, & Marslen-Wilson, 2004;Rubenstein et al, 1970). This contrasts with polysemes, for which a single written and spoken form is associated with multiple related interpretations, which may reduce the degree to which each individual meaning may be differentially activated (Armstrong & Plaut, 2008Beretta, Fiorentino, & Poeppel, 2005;Frazier & Rayner, 1990;Klepousniotou et al, 2008;Pylkkänen, Llinás, & Murphy, 2006;Rodd et al, 2002; but see Hino et al, 2010;Hino et al, 2006;Klein & Murphy, 2001.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%