2011
DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2011.0829
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Masculine voices signal men's threat potential in forager and industrial societies

Abstract: Humans and many non-human primates exhibit large sexual dimorphisms in vocalizations and vocal anatomy. In humans, same-sex competitors and potential mates attend to acoustic features of male vocalizations, but vocal masculinity especially increases perceptions of physical prowess. Yet, the information content of male vocalizations remains obscure. We therefore examined relationships between sexually dimorphic acoustic properties and men's threat potential. We first introduce a new measure of the structure of … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

13
307
6
1

Year Published

2012
2012
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 236 publications
(327 citation statements)
references
References 53 publications
13
307
6
1
Order By: Relevance
“…A final possibility is that from the perceptive of a female listener, any male voice with a fundamental frequency that is within the normal range is low enough to be perceived as competent and strong in the context of an election. This hypothesis [46]. In this same vein, substantial evidence shows that higher levels of endogenous androgens (e.g.…”
Section: Results and Discussion (A) Do Listeners Prefer Female Leadermentioning
confidence: 77%
“…A final possibility is that from the perceptive of a female listener, any male voice with a fundamental frequency that is within the normal range is low enough to be perceived as competent and strong in the context of an election. This hypothesis [46]. In this same vein, substantial evidence shows that higher levels of endogenous androgens (e.g.…”
Section: Results and Discussion (A) Do Listeners Prefer Female Leadermentioning
confidence: 77%
“…That is, motion paired with emitted sounds may satisfy the input conditions of adaptations for picking up on invariant sound/object co-relations, irrespective of whether the object is a living thing vocalizing, or a non-living object producing noise [27][28][29][30][31][32][33][34]54]. However, because the acoustics produced by the living and non-living worlds are interestingly different from one another, both in terms of their properties and also in their affordances [9,16,22,23], the psychological mechanisms for representing and reasoning about the living versus the non-living worlds will likely become increasingly distinct from one another over the course of development. Future research will therefore be needed to fully establish when and how this happens, and theoretical analyses will be needed to more fully establish the evidentiary standards of specialized design [55] for acoustic perception adaptations designed around the living versus non-living world.…”
Section: (A) Implicationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, lowering of F n and/or F 0 in dog barks increases attributions of the dog's aggressiveness in human listeners [20]. Playback studies with male deer vocalizations also show that male deer perceive the experimental lowering of F n and F 0 as more threatening ( [21]; see also [9,11,16,22,23] for more extensive reviews). Adult domestic dogs also correctly match the growls of larger versus smaller dogs onto visual cues of dog body size [24].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The lack of a relationship between pubertal timing and the PC onto which vocal dominance loaded was unexpected, as evidence indicates that voices have been shaped by sexual selection and contribute to men's dominance and mating success (Puts et al 2006(Puts et al , 2007(Puts et al , 2012aHodges-Simeon et al 2011;Wolff and Puts 2010). In addition, pubertal hormones influence vocal anatomy and acoustic parameters that contribute to perceptions of men's dominance (Fitch and Giedd 1999;Newman et al 2000;Harries et al 1998;Hollien et al 1994;Titze 2000;Hodges-Simeon et al 2013.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Participants were 153 men (mean age=20.1±1.8 year) from a large Midwestern U.S. university, selected from the 176 participants reported in Puts et al (2012a) on the basis of having provided data on pubertal timing. Five hundred sixty-eight male raters (mean age: 19.4±1.8 year) were recruited from a large northeastern U.S. university to rate facial photographs and voice recordings of the male participants.…”
Section: Methods Participantsmentioning
confidence: 99%