2016
DOI: 10.30535/mto.22.3.3
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Measuring a Measure

Abstract: The prevailing approach to bar lengths in pop/rock music uses the standard rock drum beat as a model, whereby the kick is assigned to beats 1 and 3 and the snare to beats 2 and 4 in a bar of ".fn_meter(4,4).". In this paper, I show that a song’s drum pattern is not a reliable indicator of measure lengths, especially if we consider bar lengths to be an important benchmark for theories of form. I argue that our determinations of bar lengths and meter in popular music should also take absolute time into considera… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
3
1

Year Published

2020
2020
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 20 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 18 publications
0
3
1
Order By: Relevance
“…These three songs alone do not provide enough evidence to refute the relative hypothesis, but they serve to exemplify a contrary view that I have espoused in previous work (de Clercq, 2016). According to this view, chord durations in popular music generally have a moderate pacing as measured in absolute time, regardless of the tempo implied by the drum pattern, with about 2 s as the average chord length.…”
contrasting
confidence: 59%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…These three songs alone do not provide enough evidence to refute the relative hypothesis, but they serve to exemplify a contrary view that I have espoused in previous work (de Clercq, 2016). According to this view, chord durations in popular music generally have a moderate pacing as measured in absolute time, regardless of the tempo implied by the drum pattern, with about 2 s as the average chord length.…”
contrasting
confidence: 59%
“…Preferred tempo presumably lies somewhere in the middle of this range, although the exact rate may be contingent on context. While earlier work has identified preferred tempo as approximately 100 bpm (Fraisse, 1982; Parncutt, 1994), more recent work focusing primarily on popular music suggests that preferred tempo may lie closer to 120 bpm (de Clercq, 2016; Moelants, 2002). These details aside, the interaction of absolute time, tempo, and meter undoubtedly shapes the hierarchy of regular rhythmic values in a musical work (London, 2012).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While octave errors in tempo perception occur in a wide range of musical styles, we conclude that backbeat shifts are a particularly strong means of evoking a tempo octave error. Similarly, Trevor de Clercq (2016) argued that measures (and by extension, the metrical hierarchy) in popular music are defined by durational boundaries of approximately 2 s. While de Clercq’s argument stems from a perspective of music analysis rather than perception, it nevertheless supports the notion that preference rules and cues for metric structure should be style-specific. While we would not suggest that the backbeat is an example of a “contrametric” style of organization (Kolinski, 1973; London et al, 2017), as it is a consistent, periodic rhythmic layer within the music, we recognize the relationship of the backbeat to the sub-Saharan rhythms from which it is derived that are both noncongruent with the metrical framework and fundamental to the musical style.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…The BPM and metre columns of Tables 1 and 2 provide the necessary clarification. Trevor de Clercq has raised questions concerning the reliability of determining a song's tempo from the drummer's backbeat pattern – for example, a drummer may make extensive use of a half-time feel, or a double-time feel (2016, paragraph 3.5). Richard Marx's song ‘Another One Down’ (2019) presents an ambiguous case in this regard.…”
Section: A New Phrase Modelmentioning
confidence: 99%