2019
DOI: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2019.04.010
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Measuring automatic associations between relaxing/energizing feelings and odors

Abstract: The extent to which automatic associations exist between relaxing and energizing feelings and odors is unclear. To investigate this question, we used a modified version of the Implicit Association Test. In this task, participants had to make speeded discrimination responses between stimuli and words related to energizing vs. relaxing feelings. These stimuli were either visual stimuli (i.e., vanilla vs. mint labels in Experiment 1) or olfactory (vanillin vs. menthol in Experiment 2, and two fine fragrances in E… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
13
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 20 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 39 publications
0
13
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Notably, in Yue et al (2016), participants produced longer durations (short sound intervals) when they were exposed to lavender odor (considered to be relaxing) compared to garlic and jasmine odor. One possible explanation for such results is that peppermint can also be implicitly considered as stimulating (Lemercier-Talbot et al, 2019). Moreover, Schreuder et al did not interpret the effect of the rosemary odor on the basis of an arousal-based mechanism since no differences were observed in physiological measurements.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Notably, in Yue et al (2016), participants produced longer durations (short sound intervals) when they were exposed to lavender odor (considered to be relaxing) compared to garlic and jasmine odor. One possible explanation for such results is that peppermint can also be implicitly considered as stimulating (Lemercier-Talbot et al, 2019). Moreover, Schreuder et al did not interpret the effect of the rosemary odor on the basis of an arousal-based mechanism since no differences were observed in physiological measurements.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The activation properties of odors are now considered major features of olfactory-elicited emotions in addition to their relative (un)pleasantness, according to Russell's classic bidimensional model (i.e., arousal dimension; e.g., Bensafi et al, 2002). This is also reflected within more recent multidimensional approaches (Baccarani, Brand, Dacremont, Valentin, & Brochard, 2020;Chrea et al, 2009;Delplanque et al, 2012;Ferdenzi et al, 2013;Lemercier-Talbot et al, 2019;Porcherot et al, 2010). However, the relaxing and stimulating effects of odors have been much less investigated than their hedonic valence and little is yet known about their underlying mechanisms (Herz, 2009).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…During the delivery of odor, air valves were automatically closed and odor valves opened. As typically performed in our implicit olfactory procedures (Lemercier-Talbot et al, 2019 ; Cereghetti et al, 2020 ), the interstimulus interval and the odorant flow rate were both fixed at 2 L × min −1 , thus delivering a constant flow to the nose of participants. Stainless steel tips were used as the final delivery piece.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The examples of these actuation are listening to music [85]- [88], adjusting the light in workplaces [89]- [91], smelling fragrances [92]- [94], and drinking beverages such as coffee, tea, and water [95]. Using different actuators and wearable devices, one may perform system identification and model the actuation dynamics [96]- [100]. Considering subject-specific responses and possible latency in skin conductance responses to any actuation [101], one may model the actuation dynamics and include them in the experimental WMI architectures.…”
Section: Future Directionsmentioning
confidence: 99%