2020
DOI: 10.1101/2020.05.14.092189
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Measuring Metacognition of Direct and Indirect Parameters of Voluntary Movement

Abstract: were supported by DFG, German Research Foundation (project number 222641018 -SFB/TTR 135). Data, code and pre-registration protocols are available at https://osf.io/kyhu7/ (Experiment 1) and https://osf.io/sy342/ (Experiment 2). The data discussed in this article were first published in "The Confidence Database", https://osf.io/s46pr/ (Rahnev et al., 2020). We have no known conflict of interest to disclose. AbstractWe can make exquisitely precise movements without the apparent need for conscious monitoring.But… Show more

Help me understand this report
View published versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
18
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

2
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(25 citation statements)
references
References 74 publications
5
18
0
Order By: Relevance
“…We suggest that confidence judgments about agency should be considered as the metacognitive level of an agency processing hierarchy, with agency judgments as explicit first-order judgments. This also brings agency in line with recent motor metacognition research that considers agency-like judgments such as decisions of which trajectory was caused by one's movement to be the first-order motor judgments, followed by metacognitive confidence ratings 43 .…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 71%
“…We suggest that confidence judgments about agency should be considered as the metacognitive level of an agency processing hierarchy, with agency judgments as explicit first-order judgments. This also brings agency in line with recent motor metacognition research that considers agency-like judgments such as decisions of which trajectory was caused by one's movement to be the first-order motor judgments, followed by metacognitive confidence ratings 43 .…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 71%
“…Confidence has typically been studied in two-alternative choice tasks, and only rarely in relation to continuous outcomes 30,34,36,[74][75][76] . By reconceptualizing error detection as outcome prediction, our results shed new light on the well-supported claim that error monitoring and confidence are tightly intertwined 32,38,[77][78][79] and forge valuable links between research on performance monitoring 39,40,67 and on learning under uncertainty 1,3,58,80 .…”
Section: Sensory Prediction Error and Confidence Modulate P3amentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In contrast to what we described for facial muscles, young, healthy participants have above-chance and precise metacognitive access to movements that are controlled by skeletal muscles 40 . Moreover, unlike the case of metacognition of facial expressions, measures of metacognitive performance in motor control do partially correlate with those from a visual task 41 . Speculatively, at least two factors may explain these discrepancies.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 86%
“…Participants make a movement (zero-order), then make a (first-order) judgment about it, and finally provide a (secondorder) subjective confidence rating. Examples of a zero-order task include moving a finger at a given pace 40 or throwing a ball to hit a target 41 . A different approach, which we took here, consists in operationalizing the metacognitive judgment not as confidence in accuracy of a binary choice, but instead as a judgment of performance [43][44][45] .…”
Section: Relationship To Other Metacognitive Tasksmentioning
confidence: 99%