2002
DOI: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00067-2
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Mechanisms underlying perseveration in aphasia: evidence from a single case study

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
44
0

Year Published

2006
2006
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 38 publications
(44 citation statements)
references
References 72 publications
(143 reference statements)
0
44
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Cohen & Dehaene, 1998;Martin, Roach, Brecher, & Lowery, 1998;Plaut & Shallice, 1993). In line with this account, Gotts, Incisa della Rocchetta, and Cipolotti (2002) found that factors that weaken or slow the processing of a current stimulus, such as low-lexical frequency, give rise to more perseverations. However, given this balance between 'new' and 'old' activations, it seems more appropriate, for Wernicke's patients at least, to stress the abnormally high persisting activation because of deWcient activation suppression.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 70%
“…Cohen & Dehaene, 1998;Martin, Roach, Brecher, & Lowery, 1998;Plaut & Shallice, 1993). In line with this account, Gotts, Incisa della Rocchetta, and Cipolotti (2002) found that factors that weaken or slow the processing of a current stimulus, such as low-lexical frequency, give rise to more perseverations. However, given this balance between 'new' and 'old' activations, it seems more appropriate, for Wernicke's patients at least, to stress the abnormally high persisting activation because of deWcient activation suppression.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 70%
“…Consequently, any factor that weakens the intended linguistic unit, or contributes to that of a prior unit promotes the probability of a perseveration. For example, a low-frequency word target should be vulnerable to perseverations of high-frequency words that were recently spoken, and data indicate that this is the case (Gotts, della Rocchetta, & Cipolotti, 2002;Hirsh, 1998). Moreover, the probability of a perseveration should be further enhanced if the two share semantic or phonological features, and again the data are supportive (Martin et al, 1998).…”
Section: Common Mechanisms In Substitutionsmentioning
confidence: 88%
“…However, several studies have demonstrated a systematic time-lag relationship between a target word and the most recent occurrence of a word that displaces that target (Cohen & Dehane, 1998;Gotts et al, 2002). This systematic relationship between the temporal window separating target and potential perseverate and the probability of that perseveration occurring is more easily reconciled with a systematic decaying function of activation potential than momentary failure of inhibition.…”
Section: Common Mechanisms In Substitutionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While these two studies suggest selective vulnerability to the blocking effect in patients with anterior forms of aphasia, the handful of other patient studies that utilized blocked naming have mostly produced negative effects for aphasics of both anterior and posterior types (Gotts, della Rocchetta, & Cipolotti, 2002;Hodgson et al, 2003;negative in one of two patients tested, Lambon Ralph, Sage, & Roberts, 2000;Schwartz & Hodgson, 2002;Williams & Wright, 1985). Thus, the literature raises questions about the generality of the findings from patients FAS and BM and the significance of the fact that their aphasias were of the anterior type (i.e., featuring nonfluent propositional speech with or without agrammatism).…”
Section: Semantic Blocking In Speakers With Aphasiamentioning
confidence: 98%