2016
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.03.007
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Metacognitive developments in word learning: Mutual exclusivity and theory of mind

Abstract: This study examines the flexibility with which children can use pragmatic information to determine word reference. Extensive previous research shows that children choose an unfamiliar object as referent of a novel name: the disambiguation effect. We added a pragmatic cue indirectly indicating a familiar object as intended referent. In three experiments, preschool children's ability to take this cue into account was specifically associated with false belief understanding and the ability to produce familiar alte… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
12
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(15 citation statements)
references
References 35 publications
3
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Three-year-olds tended to select the novel object, but 5-year-olds tended to select the familiar object. Gollek and Doherty (2016) found a similar difference between 3- and 4-year-olds in a study of Austrian children and similar trends in two studies of 3- and 4-year-old Scottish children.…”
supporting
confidence: 78%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Three-year-olds tended to select the novel object, but 5-year-olds tended to select the familiar object. Gollek and Doherty (2016) found a similar difference between 3- and 4-year-olds in a study of Austrian children and similar trends in two studies of 3- and 4-year-old Scottish children.…”
supporting
confidence: 78%
“…Gollek and Doherty (2016) proposed that unless a child understood that two labels can express different perspectives on the same object, they would be reluctant to satisfy the pragmatic cue by overriding their usual tendency to avoid mapping a novel label to an object with a known label. In support of this proposal, they found that in each of their studies, performance on tests of understanding of multiple perspective (e.g., understanding of false belief) predicted selection of the familiar object as the referent of the novel label, even after the effects of age and vocabulary size were statistically controlled.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These findings suggest that the use of pictorial information in jigsaw puzzle completion reflects the development of a general concept of representation, which is evident in the mental, graphical and pictorial domain. This adds to the body of literature showing linked metarepresentational developments across the linguistic, mental, and pictorial domains (Diaz & Farrar, 2018;Doherty & Perner, 1998;Doherty & Wimmer, 2005;Gollek, & Doherty, 2016;Perner & Roessler, 2012;Perner et al, 2002;Wimmer & Doherty, 2011). Specifically, we suggest that children begin to understand that pictorial elements, such as lines and patches of color, can be arranged to produce a picture.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 56%
“…The emergence of this ability around 4 years explains why these tasks are mastered at this age and strongly correlate. This has been demonstrated in eleven published experiments, total N = 513, 0.53 ≤ r ≤ 0.77; 0.29 ≤ r p ≤ 0.59 with age, or age and verbal IQ controlled (Diaz & Farrar, 2018;Doherty, 2000;Doherty & Perner, 1998;Gollek & Doherty, 2016;Perner et al, 2002;Wimmer & Doherty, 2011).…”
Section: Perspectivementioning
confidence: 79%
“…This prediction was examined by Gollek and Doherty (2016), who simply gave 2-to 5-year-old children the false belief task, alternative naming task, and the pragmatic-cue version of the disambiguation task. All three were strongly associated over three experiments (all r between 0.40 and 0.69), even beyond common associations with age, verbal mental age, and inhibitory ability (all partial r between 0.35 and 0.53).…”
Section: Predictionsmentioning
confidence: 99%