1953
DOI: 10.1364/josa.43.000648
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Metacontrast

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2

Citation Types

10
229
5

Year Published

1969
1969
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
5
3
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 337 publications
(244 citation statements)
references
References 16 publications
10
229
5
Order By: Relevance
“…The first ingredient in the sensory explanation, then, is that spaces are more effective in the periphery. Alpern (1953) postulated that there was increased contour interaction in the periphery. A space in the periphery, then.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The first ingredient in the sensory explanation, then, is that spaces are more effective in the periphery. Alpern (1953) postulated that there was increased contour interaction in the periphery. A space in the periphery, then.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As set size is increased, the mask representation is assumed to become stronger whereas the target representation remains the same. We believe this modeling of attention to be inappropriate.It has long been known that the strength of metacontrast masking is directly related to mask intensity (Alpern, 1953). It is, therefore, unsurprising that weakening the mask reduces the metacontrast effect in all extant models.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This procedure assured that, except for onset and offset, the mask as well as the target were activating only sustained channels. However, under these conditions, neither of the observers reported any disappearance or decrease in the visi- (Alpern, 1953;Kolers & Rosner, 1960;Weisstein & Growney, 1969). The interaction between target-mask spatial separation and viewing eccentricity was also statistically significant for both observers' results [B.B., F(6,84)=6.5; M.K., F(6,84)=7.8; ps < .005].…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 83%