2010
DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2010.0586
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Mimics without models: causes and consequences of allopatry in Batesian mimicry complexes

Abstract: Batesian mimicry evolves when a palatable species (the 'mimic') co-opts a warning signal from a dangerous species (the 'model') and thereby deceives its potential predators. Longstanding theory predicts that this protection from predation should break down where the model is absent. Thus, mimics are expected to only co-occur with their model. Yet, many mimics violate this prediction and occur in areas where their model is absent. Here, we discuss the causes and consequences of such allopatric mimics. We also d… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

2
54
0

Year Published

2011
2011
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 62 publications
(56 citation statements)
references
References 65 publications
2
54
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Both Endler & Rojas (2009) and Sherratt (2006) show that this breakdown in stabilizing selection can facilitate the evolution of novel aposematic phenotypes because of the ‘flat’ adaptive landscape associated with regions of low signal frequency. Therefore, spatial or temporal variation in the local density of aposematic phenotypes may facilitate the evolution of novel signals as a result of coincident fluctuations in adaptive landscapes (Sherratt, 2006; Endler & Rojas, 2009); however, this process lacks empirical support (however, see Pfennig & Mullen, 2010 for an example related to Batesian mimicry).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Both Endler & Rojas (2009) and Sherratt (2006) show that this breakdown in stabilizing selection can facilitate the evolution of novel aposematic phenotypes because of the ‘flat’ adaptive landscape associated with regions of low signal frequency. Therefore, spatial or temporal variation in the local density of aposematic phenotypes may facilitate the evolution of novel signals as a result of coincident fluctuations in adaptive landscapes (Sherratt, 2006; Endler & Rojas, 2009); however, this process lacks empirical support (however, see Pfennig & Mullen, 2010 for an example related to Batesian mimicry).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Regardless of why mimicry fails to break down in allopatry, these findings have important implications for Batesian mimicry's role in local adaptation and, potentially, speciation (Pfennig & Mullen, ; Pfennig et al ., ; Davis Rabosky et al ., ). Generally, populations of mimics that occur in sympatry with their model vs. those in allopatry are expected to experience contrasting selective pressures (Pfennig et al ., ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Second, the precision of mimics has been shown to decrease in locations where their model is absent; that is, in allopatry (Harper & Pfennig, ; Ries & Mullen, ). Although long‐standing theory predicts that mimics should only occur in regions inhabited by their model (Wallace, ; Ruxton et al ., ), many species of mimics violate this prediction and also occupy regions where their model is absent (Pfennig & Mullen, ). Although mimics might benefit from being conspicuous in the absence of models if, for instance, predators innately avoid such signals, mimics that occur in such locations are generally expected to experience selection against mimetic phenotypes because such phenotypes are typically aposematic and thus conspicuous to potential predators (Ruxton et al ., ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As yet, we are unaware of studies outside of egg mimicry in birds where the costs and benefits of mimicry to all protagonists have been calculated in terms of reproductive output – the currency of selection. However, there are some excellent experimental proofs of concept in Batesian systems involving snakes (Pfennig et al ., ; Pfennig & Mullen, ), plants (Johnson, Alexandersson, & Linder, ), bats (Barber & Conner, ), flies (de Jager & Ellis, ) and wasps (Wong & Shiestl, ), where the costs and benefits have been determined for one or two of the protagonists. Furthermore, we suggest that disentangling the components of selection resulting from the costs/benefits of mimicry versus purifying selection will give much more insight into how mimicry evolves.…”
Section: Strength Balance and Opposing Forces Of Selectionmentioning
confidence: 99%