2021
DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2021.101421
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Mind the gap: How incomplete explanations influence children’s interest and learning behaviors

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
8
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 16 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 58 publications
0
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Children completed a number of measures as a part of a battery for a larger project supported by the National Science Foundation (Danovitch et al, 2021 ). Of particular interest for this study, children completed a biological knowledge measure ( N = 148) and the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test—Second Edition (KBIT‐2; N = 71).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Children completed a number of measures as a part of a battery for a larger project supported by the National Science Foundation (Danovitch et al, 2021 ). Of particular interest for this study, children completed a biological knowledge measure ( N = 148) and the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test—Second Edition (KBIT‐2; N = 71).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Evidence suggests associations between curiosity and achievement at school entry ( Shah et al, 2018 ) and that curiosity supports academic performance, even when controlling for students’ effort and ability ( von Stumm et al, 2011 ). Despite this evidence, most prior research on the development of curiosity or on promoting curiosity has been conducted in lab settings with individual children (e.g., Cook et al, 2011 ; Gweon et al, 2014 ; Shneidman et al, 2016 ; Danovitch et al, 2021 among others), rather than in school settings. In research that did look at promoting curiosity in an educational context, researchers test specific manipulations with researchers administering the lesson to promote curiosity in schools (e.g., Lamnina and Chase, 2019 ) or parents in a museum setting (e.g., Willard et al, 2019 ), or observed children’s exploration without studying instruction or promotion of curiosity (e.g., van Schijndel et al, 2018 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For instance, prompting children to explain observations in a novel domain leads them to ask more informative questions and thus solve a causal learning task in that domain with greater efficiency (Ruggeri et al, 2019). In receiving explanations, young children favor informants who provide noncircular explanations (Corriveau & Kurkul, 2014) and are more likely to seek more information following an unsatisfying or incomplete explanation (Danovitch et al, 2021; Mills et al, 2017), suggesting that explanatory phenomenology directs how and when inquiry is pursued.…”
Section: Indirect Benefits Of Explanatory Selectivitymentioning
confidence: 99%