2017
DOI: 10.1111/tops.12308
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Mindreading and Psycholinguistic Approaches to Perspective Taking: Establishing Common Ground

Abstract: In this commentary on "Memory and Common Ground Processes in Language Use," I draw attention to relevant work on mindreading. The concerns of research on common ground and mindreading have significant overlap, but these literatures have worked in relative isolation of each other. I attempt an assimilation, pointing out shared and distinctive concerns and mutually informative results.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
17
0
2

Year Published

2019
2019
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 18 publications
(20 citation statements)
references
References 30 publications
1
17
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…As mentioned earlier, a component-based account (sampling, maintaining, deploying) is consistent with distinctions others have made about the component processes underlying perspective-taking during language comprehension (Apperly, 2018). It is also consistent with one interpretation of perspective-taking failures demonstrated in previous findings--namely, that apparent failures to adopt a speaker's perspective when inferring their intended interpretation are due primarily to failures to successfully deploy this information, rather than encode it in the first place (Keysar, 2000;Keysar et al, 2003).…”
supporting
confidence: 83%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…As mentioned earlier, a component-based account (sampling, maintaining, deploying) is consistent with distinctions others have made about the component processes underlying perspective-taking during language comprehension (Apperly, 2018). It is also consistent with one interpretation of perspective-taking failures demonstrated in previous findings--namely, that apparent failures to adopt a speaker's perspective when inferring their intended interpretation are due primarily to failures to successfully deploy this information, rather than encode it in the first place (Keysar, 2000;Keysar et al, 2003).…”
supporting
confidence: 83%
“…These processes roughly correspond to what Apperly (2018) labels inference, storage, and use. As Apperly (2018) notes, individual differences among comprehenders may produce variability in their ability to successfully engage each process: "the likelihood that others' mental states will be inferred, stored, or used depends upon dispositional characteristics of the participants, their MENTALIZING FOR PRAGMATIC INFERENCE 7 motivation, and their cognitive resources" (Apperly, 2018, pg. 6).…”
Section: A Mechanistic Explanation: Different Component Processes Submentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The present results also have broader implications for our understanding of social cognition. Much research has been concerned with the question of how theory of mind allows us to overcome the gap between our own and others' minds and contributes to building common ground (e.g., Apperly, 2018; Schaafsma et al, 2015). While many social interactions indeed require the ability to attribute mental states to others and to keep them apart from one's own, it is interesting to note that coordination in joint action may be achieved through joint action plans in the form of We-representations.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…But in the absence of such motivation there is no evidence at all that beliefs are inferred. " Apperly (2010" Apperly ( , 2018 also claims that experimental paradigms tapping the derivation of pragmatic inferences have in-built incentives to motivate participants to engage in pragmatic reasoning. For example, McKoon and Ratcliff (1986) asked participants to read sentences such as 'The woman, desperate to get away, ran to the car and jumped in' and then later decide whether the word 'driving' had appeared in the sentence.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%