2003
DOI: 10.1353/cjl.2004.0027
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Missing Inflection in L2 Acquisition: Defective Syntax or L1-Constrained Prosodic Representations?

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

6
108
3
4

Year Published

2007
2007
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
2

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 92 publications
(121 citation statements)
references
References 37 publications
6
108
3
4
Order By: Relevance
“…The successful production of overt subjects is replicated in other existing studies (Lardiere, 1998 a, b;Haznedar, 2001;Inonin and Wexler, 2002;White, 2003;Goad, White and Steele, 2003), regardless of the fact that the L2 learners' L1 33 permits null subjects.…”
Section: Subject Raising and Case Markingsupporting
confidence: 68%
“…The successful production of overt subjects is replicated in other existing studies (Lardiere, 1998 a, b;Haznedar, 2001;Inonin and Wexler, 2002;White, 2003;Goad, White and Steele, 2003), regardless of the fact that the L2 learners' L1 33 permits null subjects.…”
Section: Subject Raising and Case Markingsupporting
confidence: 68%
“…After 10 years' residence in the United States, Patty's production of obligatory inflections for past tense -ed and third person singular -s was extremely deficient (34 % and 5 % production, respectively); 8 years later, there was no improvement. Similarly, Goad, White, and Steele (2003) reported low production of tense and agreement morphology in L1 Mandarin Chinese late learners' descriptions of pictures (length of residence in Canada = 6 months to 5 years).…”
Section: The Challenge Of Inflections For L1 Chinesementioning
confidence: 92%
“…High production (about 90 %) of be variants (is, was) and of related verbal forms (has, have) does in fact occur in the spoken and written English of L1 Chinese (Goad et al, 2003;Lardière, 1998aLardière, , 1998bLardière, , 2003Lardière, , 2006. On the basis of such findings, it has been proposed that late learners possess intact mental representations for tense and agreement, but may be unable to access these representations and/ or procedures for integrating inflections with syntactic features when resources for processing the L2 are stressed (Prévost & White, 2000;White, 2010; see also Hopp, 2010).…”
Section: The Challenge Of Inflections For L1 Chinesementioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Control over the feature requires knowledge of both the irregular and regular forms, but systematic use of the irregular past (did, said, went) tends to occur before productive use of the regular past (Lee, 2001;Rohde, 1996). At least some of the difficulty derives from the lack of perceptual salience of the regular form of the simple past (Bell, Trofimovich, & Collins, in press;McDonald & Roussel, 2010; see also Bayley, 1996;Goad, White, & Steele, 2003;Wolfram, 1985). In addition, learners initially make much more frequent use of the simple past with the class of verbs known as telics (finished, made something), which are bounded events with inherent end points.…”
Section: Simple Past and Possessive Determiners (Pds) His/hermentioning
confidence: 99%