2016
DOI: 10.1016/j.robot.2016.08.025
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Modeling incubation and restructuring for creative problem solving in robots

Abstract: Creativity represents the pinnacle of higher-level cognition, but exactly how it is achieved remains poorly understood, especially when simultaneously facing the opposing challenge of intractable complexity. The aims of the current study were (a) to examine how the brain may achieve the dual goals of creativity and complexity reduction, and (b) to begin developing higher-level cognition and creativity in robots. We addressed these aims by (a) modeling an example of insight problem solving and comparing it to e… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
5

Citation Types

0
22
0
1

Year Published

2018
2018
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
3
1
1

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(23 citation statements)
references
References 24 publications
0
22
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, to find a new path to a restaurant one normally considers the most obvious means of transportation (e.g., walk, subway, car), and will take the most direct route available; these solutions are in turn bound by the factors of cost, availability, and intended effort. In contrast, creative problem-solving entails consideration of nonapparent problem components, which at first pass means those outside the scope of the original problem representation: i.e., those not as salient, directly relevant, or learned from experience (Cheng, Ray, Nguyen, & Kralik, 2013; Kralik, Mao, Cheng, & Ray, 2016; Kralik, Shi, & El-Shroa, 2016; Smith & Ward, 2012). A classic example with humans is the 9-dot problem, in which nine dots are displayed in a 3 × 3 square matrix, and the participant must connect all nine dots by drawing only four lines without lifting the pen/pencil (Cheng et al, 2013; Maier, 1930; van Steenburgh et al, 2012).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 4 more Smart Citations
“…For example, to find a new path to a restaurant one normally considers the most obvious means of transportation (e.g., walk, subway, car), and will take the most direct route available; these solutions are in turn bound by the factors of cost, availability, and intended effort. In contrast, creative problem-solving entails consideration of nonapparent problem components, which at first pass means those outside the scope of the original problem representation: i.e., those not as salient, directly relevant, or learned from experience (Cheng, Ray, Nguyen, & Kralik, 2013; Kralik, Mao, Cheng, & Ray, 2016; Kralik, Shi, & El-Shroa, 2016; Smith & Ward, 2012). A classic example with humans is the 9-dot problem, in which nine dots are displayed in a 3 × 3 square matrix, and the participant must connect all nine dots by drawing only four lines without lifting the pen/pencil (Cheng et al, 2013; Maier, 1930; van Steenburgh et al, 2012).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In this case, the highly salient dots and most direct lines that begin and end at the dots define the apparent problem formulation, whereas a solution can only be found when one realizes that the lines can extend past the dots ( nonapparent formulation). Thus, in creative problem-solving, an inadequate formulation of the problem based on apparent factors must be replaced by considering ‘outside-the-box’ components (Kralik, Mao, Cheng, & Ray, 2016; Kralik, Shi, & El-Shroa, 2016; Smith & Ward, 2012; van Steenburgh et al, 2012).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations