2018
DOI: 10.31234/osf.io/9qkhj
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Modeling Monotonic Effects of Ordinal Predictors in Bayesian Regression Models

Abstract: Ordinal predictors are commonly used in regression models. They are often incorrectly treated as either nominal or metric, thus under- or overestimating the contained information. Such practices may lead to worse inference and predictions compared to methods which are specifically designed for this purpose. We propose a new method for modeling ordinal predictors that applies in situations in which it is reasonable to assume their effects to be monotonic. The parameterization of such monotonic effects is realiz… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
21
0
1

Year Published

2019
2019
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 29 publications
(22 citation statements)
references
References 44 publications
(91 reference statements)
0
21
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The main fixed effects were response modality (spoken, keyboard, grid) Contour A and B sequences, vertical broken lines mark group boundaries (intonational phrases), whereas in the grouped-by-pause sequence, double vertical broken lines mark silent intervals (pauses) between groups and condition (Intonation A, Intonation B, pause, control). In addition, we included a fixed effect for "position within triplet," which was added as a monotonic variable (see Bürkner & Charpentier, 2018). This variable codes for the first, second, and third position within each triplet (1, 2, 3 versus 4, 5, 6 versus 7, 8, 9).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The main fixed effects were response modality (spoken, keyboard, grid) Contour A and B sequences, vertical broken lines mark group boundaries (intonational phrases), whereas in the grouped-by-pause sequence, double vertical broken lines mark silent intervals (pauses) between groups and condition (Intonation A, Intonation B, pause, control). In addition, we included a fixed effect for "position within triplet," which was added as a monotonic variable (see Bürkner & Charpentier, 2018). This variable codes for the first, second, and third position within each triplet (1, 2, 3 versus 4, 5, 6 versus 7, 8, 9).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This was a change from the preregistered script (https://osf.io/dt2uq/), in which we did the analyses with dummy data with the package lme4 (Bates, Maechler, Bolker, & Walker, 2015). The change was due to the advantage of brms in handling ordinal predictors (Bürkner & Charpentier, 2018). This change did not affect the qualitative conclusions derived from the results (see SM2 and SM3).…”
Section: 6mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We first ran a generation model with generation (one to four) as the sole predictor of recall (number of correctly recalled propositions). Generation was treated as a monotonic variable (Bürkner & Charpentier, 2018) as recall tends to decrease across generations but the amount of the decrease varies between adjacent generations. This model was compared with an opinion model, which included opinion ('pro-tablets' vs 'anti-tablets') and generation as predictors.…”
Section: Cumulative Recallmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…mcp supports a scaled and shifted Dirichlet prior on the difference(s) between adjacent change points -an approach that is also used to model monotonic effects in brms (Bürkner and Charpentier, 2018). The Dirichlet is itself a simplex: ζ k > 0 ensures ordering and while the support is ζ k ∈ (0, 1) the property that K−1 k=1 ζ k = 1 renders it easy to shift and scale the support to the observed range of x ([min(x), max(x)]).…”
Section: Dirichlet Priormentioning
confidence: 99%