2000
DOI: 10.1007/s11881-000-0017-3
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Modeling the response of normally achieving and at-risk first grade children to word reading instruction

Abstract: The purpose of this study was to identify important subject characteristics that predicted individual differences in responsiveness to word reading instruction in normally achieving and at-risk first grade children. This was accomplished by modeling individual word and nonword reading growth, and the correlates of change in these skills, in first grade students during two different phases of the school year. In the first phase of the study (October-January), word and nonword reading skill was modeled in normal… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
20
0
2

Year Published

2006
2006
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 30 publications
(23 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
1
20
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…This research contributes to an applied body of work developed in response to accumulating evidence showing that children who enter kindergarten with well-developed language and literacy skills perform better in beginning reading instruction relative to those with less-developed skills, the latter facing substantial risk for timely achievement of skilled and fluent reading (e.g., Compton, 2000;O'Connor & Jenkins, 1999). Participation in preschool programs providing high quality language and literacy instruction is considered one of the most viable mechanisms for improving at-risk children's transition to reading instruction and reducing their vulnerability for later reading difficulties (see Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This research contributes to an applied body of work developed in response to accumulating evidence showing that children who enter kindergarten with well-developed language and literacy skills perform better in beginning reading instruction relative to those with less-developed skills, the latter facing substantial risk for timely achievement of skilled and fluent reading (e.g., Compton, 2000;O'Connor & Jenkins, 1999). Participation in preschool programs providing high quality language and literacy instruction is considered one of the most viable mechanisms for improving at-risk children's transition to reading instruction and reducing their vulnerability for later reading difficulties (see Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Phonological awareness (Liberman et al, 1974;Snowling, 2000), rapid naming (Compton, 2000;Compton et al, 2001;Manis et al, 2000) and letter knowledge (Pennington and Lefly, 2001;Puolakanaho et al, 2007) measured in kindergarteners correlated with reading in 2 nd grade and together explained 51% of the variance. While RAN significantly improved classification of poor and normal reading 2 nd graders, letter knowledge and phonological awareness did not contribute to this discrimination, even though letter knowledge has repeatedly been identified as an important indicator of later reading problems at preschool age (Pennington and Lefly, 2001;Puolakanaho et al, 2007), especially in consistent languages.…”
Section: Prediction Of Reading Outcome With Behavioural Measuresmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…RAN refers to the ability to quickly name visual symbols such as letters, digits, colors or objects (Denckla & Rudel, 1974) and is predictive of reading ability, including word identification, reading comprehension and phonological recoding (Ackerman & Dykman, 1993;Compton, 2000;Manis, Seidenberg, & Doi, 1999;Schatsneider, Fletcher, Francis, Carlson, & Foorman, 2004). Although RAN is an important reading skill, there is debate within the literature as to where to place RAN among the other predictors of reading.…”
Section: Reading Skillsmentioning
confidence: 99%