2015
DOI: 10.1515/tl-2015-0001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Monotonicity and the typology of front/back harmony

Abstract: In this paper, we analyse the surface patterns of suffix harmony in front/back harmony systems as the harmonic values front and back being assigned to harmonic contexts consisting of strings of syllables combining front, back and neutral nuclei. We claim that the harmonic contexts can be arranged in a fixed (universal) scale, the frontness/backness scale, with reference to which the possible (i.e. attested) front/back harmony systems can be characterised in a simple way: only those systems are possible where t… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

1
20
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 19 publications
(21 citation statements)
references
References 26 publications
1
20
0
Order By: Relevance
“…vowels ("mixed roots"), though with [ɛ] most are loanwords. This difference corresponds to the strong versus weak disharmony distinction drawn by Rebrus & Törkenczy (2015). Törkenczy (2010: 8) provides corpus counts supporting this distinction and notes that intuitively for native speakers, disharmonic roots "feel foreign", while mixed roots do not.…”
Section: Neutrality In Rootsmentioning
confidence: 93%
“…vowels ("mixed roots"), though with [ɛ] most are loanwords. This difference corresponds to the strong versus weak disharmony distinction drawn by Rebrus & Törkenczy (2015). Törkenczy (2010: 8) provides corpus counts supporting this distinction and notes that intuitively for native speakers, disharmonic roots "feel foreign", while mixed roots do not.…”
Section: Neutrality In Rootsmentioning
confidence: 93%
“…Similarly, the vacillating root of (6h) remains vacillating with the invariant neutral-vowelled possessive suffix, while the nonvacillating root of (6i) remains nonvacillating. We call this phenomenon harmonic uniformity (Rebrus & Törkenczy 2015).…”
Section: Harmonic Uniformitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, high vowels are more likely to be transparent to back vowel harmony than low vowels (Hayes and Londe 2006;Smith 2016). Rebrus and Törkenczy (2015) hypothesize that this height effect is a result of the phonological representation of transparent vowels in back vowel harmony. High vowels are phonetically more likely to be 'in-between' front and back, which allows them to pattern with both front and back vowels, thereby allowing for both harmony and disharmony.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…For example, in Hungarian, there are stems that contain a front neutral vowel that always select for a back vowel suffix (e.g., [ʃi:r-ok] 'tomb-PL' (Rebrus and Törkenczy 2015)). Rebrus and Törkenczy (2015) predict that only languages with transparent vowels allow for anti-harmony; languages with opaque vowels will not show anti-harmony. This prediction appears to be borne out for the known typology of back vowel harmony languages.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%