2006
DOI: 10.1016/j.appdev.2006.04.003
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Moral, conventional, and personal rules: The perspective of foster youth

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2

Citation Types

1
3
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
3

Relationship

0
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 3 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 55 publications
1
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This requirement to strictly follow all rules may have socialized these participants to show an overly rule-oriented response pattern. These findings are consistent with previous research on individuals with anti-social behaviours (Blair, 1995(Blair, , 1997Tisak & Jankowski, 1996) as well as children in foster care, who use conventional reasoning even when evaluating moral and personal violations (Mullins & Tisak, 2006). Furthermore, these findings are also in line with deterrence theory, suggesting that people frequently obey rules because they think they might get caught and punished for violating the rule (Nagin, 1998;Paternoster, 2010;Piquero, Paternoster, Pogarsky, & Loughran, 2011).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…This requirement to strictly follow all rules may have socialized these participants to show an overly rule-oriented response pattern. These findings are consistent with previous research on individuals with anti-social behaviours (Blair, 1995(Blair, , 1997Tisak & Jankowski, 1996) as well as children in foster care, who use conventional reasoning even when evaluating moral and personal violations (Mullins & Tisak, 2006). Furthermore, these findings are also in line with deterrence theory, suggesting that people frequently obey rules because they think they might get caught and punished for violating the rule (Nagin, 1998;Paternoster, 2010;Piquero, Paternoster, Pogarsky, & Loughran, 2011).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…Finally, our analyses did not indicate any publication bias. As our analyses included numerous studies with children of diverse ages and compared moral and conventional distinctions across different ages rather than merely examining whether participants differentiated morality from conventions (e.g., Buchanan-Barrow & Barrett, 1998; Mullins & Tisak, 2006; Smetana & Braeges, 1990; Song et al, 1987), this meta-analysis may have included studies with null or weak distinction effects. Nevertheless, as we only found and included a handful of unpublished studies and the “file drawer” problem may influence the overall effect sizes, the results should be interpreted with caution.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The role of gender has not been of central interest in this body of research, although studies have often tested for gender effects. Most studies have not found variations in moral/conventional distinctions in judgments according to child gender (e.g., Buchanan-Barrow & Barrett, 1998; Crane & Tisak, 1995; Smetana, Rote, et al, 2012), but when differences have been found, the direction of the effects (whether males make greater distinctions than females or vice versa) has been inconsistent (e.g., Caravita et al, 2017; Mullins & Tisak, 2006). Therefore, gender effects need to be tested more systematically.…”
Section: Morality As Distinct From Conventionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation