2007
DOI: 10.1080/02699930600980874
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Moral sentiments and cooperation: Differential influences of shame and guilt

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

18
213
0
6

Year Published

2011
2011
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 271 publications
(237 citation statements)
references
References 20 publications
18
213
0
6
Order By: Relevance
“…This main goal is evident in altruistic guilt and, to less extend, in deontological guilt, which has shown to be associated with feelings of shame. Although shame has been defined as a moral emotion (Frank, 1988;Ketelaar, 2004;Smith, 1759) very closely related to guilt, there is growing evidence that they are clearly distinguishable (Tangney, 1991;Tangney, 1996;De Hooge, Zeelenberg, & Breugelmans, 2007). Guilt and shame share some common characteristics, but might be distinguished on the basis of their public-private dimension, their action tendencies (hiding and escaping in shame, and confessing, apologizing or undoing the consequences of the behavior in guilt) and on their intensity (shame being described as more painful and intense) (Lewis, 1971;Tangney& Dearing, 2002;Tangney, Stuewig, & Mashek, 2007).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This main goal is evident in altruistic guilt and, to less extend, in deontological guilt, which has shown to be associated with feelings of shame. Although shame has been defined as a moral emotion (Frank, 1988;Ketelaar, 2004;Smith, 1759) very closely related to guilt, there is growing evidence that they are clearly distinguishable (Tangney, 1991;Tangney, 1996;De Hooge, Zeelenberg, & Breugelmans, 2007). Guilt and shame share some common characteristics, but might be distinguished on the basis of their public-private dimension, their action tendencies (hiding and escaping in shame, and confessing, apologizing or undoing the consequences of the behavior in guilt) and on their intensity (shame being described as more painful and intense) (Lewis, 1971;Tangney& Dearing, 2002;Tangney, Stuewig, & Mashek, 2007).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Emotions were induced with the well-validated autobiographical recall procedure followed by a selfreport manipulation check (e.g. De Hooge, 2014;De Hooge et al, 2008;De Hooge, Zeelenberg, & Breugelmans, 2007;Ketelaar & Au, 2003;Nelissen, Dijker, & De Vries, 2007). Respondents in the Shame/Guilt condition were asked to thoroughly think about, and briefly write down on a note, a personal experience in which they felt very ashamed ("schaamte" in Dutch) / very guilty ("schuld" in Dutch).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, people who are prone to feeling guilty after committing transgressions behave less aggressively when angered (Stuewig, Tangney, Heigel, Harty, & McCloskey, 2010;Tangney, Wagner, Hill-Barlow, Marschall, & Gramzow, 1996), are less likely to commit delinquent offenses as adolescents (Stuewig & McCloskey, 2005), and express more disapproval of lying in business negotiations (Cohen, 2010). Likewise, inducing people to feel guilty by having them recall past misdeeds causes them to behave more cooperatively in interpersonal interactions (de Hooge, Zeelenberg, & Breugelmans, 2007;Ketelaar & Au, 2003). Indeed, the biggest difference between individuals with antisocial personality disorder and well-adjusted individuals is the former's "inability to feel sympathy, shame, guilt, or other emotions that make the rest of us care about the fates of others and the things we do to hurt or help them" (Haidt & Kesebir, 2010, p. 804).…”
Section: Introducing the Gasp Scale: A New Measure Of Guilt And Shamementioning
confidence: 99%