126One of the primary aims of associative learning theory and the research on which it is based is to establish the factors that influence learning-the factors that determine why, under a certain set of circumstances, an organism might learn more about the consequences of Stimulus X than about those of Stimulus Y. Evidence from the field of animal conditioning suggests that one such factor is the p prior predictive history of a stimulus. In particular, previous experience of a cue (e.g., a tone) as being predictive or nonpredictive of an outcome event (e.g., food) will influence the rate of subsequent learning about that cue (e.g., Bennett, Wills, Oakeshott, & Mackintosh, 2000; Dopson, Esber, & Pearce, in press;Hall & Pearce, 1979, 1982 Haselgrove, Esber, Pearce, & Jones, in press;Lubow & Moore, 1959;Mackintosh & Little, 1969;Mackintosh & Turner, 1971;Wilson, Boumphrey, & Pearce, 1992; see Le Pelley, 2004, for a review). And consistent with the suggestion that a common associative-learning mechanism might underlie animal conditioning and human contingency learning (Dickinson, 2001), several recent studies have demonstrated an influence of prior predictiveness on the rate of learning about cues in human learning (e.g., Bonardi, Graham, Hall, & Mitchell, 2005;Griffiths & Le Pelley, 2009;Kruschke, 1996;Le Pelley & McLaren, 2003;Le Pelley, Schmidt-Hansen, Harris, Lunter, & Morris, 2010;Livesey & McLaren, 2007;Lochmann & Wills, 2003;Maes, Damen, & Eling, 2004). This rate of learning about the cue is often referred to as the cue's associability, or, perhaps more controversially, the attention paid to the cue. In this article, we remain uncommitted on exactly what aspect of cue processing is influenced by previous experience of predictiveness-specifically, whether such differences in processing truly reflect differences in attention (see Le Pelley, in press-a). Hence, from this point on, we will use the theoretically neutral label alpha to refer to that property of cue processing that is influenced by the prior predictiveness of the cue.The problem posed by the literature cited above is that f the specific way in which the learned predictiveness of h stimuli influences alpha seems very inconsistent. With regard to the animal learning literature, the results of certain studies indicate that stimuli that have previously been r experienced as reliable predictors will maintain a higher t alpha (and hence, will subsequently be learned about more rapidly) than will stimuli experienced as nonpredictive (Bennett et al., 2000; Dopson et al., in press;George & Pearce, 1999;Mackintosh, 1969;Mackintosh & Little, 1969). Such findings fit well with the theoretical stance taken by Mackintosh (1975; hereafter, referred Kruschke, 2001;Sutherland & Mackintosh, 1971). Other studies, however, seem to demonstrate exactly the opposite, showing faster learning about stimuli that have previously been unreliable predictors, as compared with those experienced as consistently predictive (e.g., Haselgrove et al., in press;Swan & Pearce, 198...