2008
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.12.010
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Morpheme-based reading aloud: Evidence from dyslexic and skilled Italian readers

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

29
171
2
15

Year Published

2009
2009
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 126 publications
(217 citation statements)
references
References 41 publications
29
171
2
15
Order By: Relevance
“…Nunes et al 1997Nunes et al , 2006Kemp 2006; however, studies are only just beginning to examine in detail how morphology maps to orthography in the development of orthographic representations. For example, Burani et al (2008) found that children were faster to read psuedowords that were made up of root and suffix morphemes (for example, womanist is a pseudoword but is made from two morphemes, namely womanþist) than pseudowords that did not contain embedded morphemes. Interestingly, younger children (and poorer readers) also showed a processing advantage for words that contained morphological structure, suggesting that they were relying on morphological parsing to a greater extent than more skilled readers (see Reichle & Perfetti (2003) and Verhoeven & Perfetti (2003) for further preliminary work in this area).…”
Section: Form -Meaning Connections and Learning To Readmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Nunes et al 1997Nunes et al , 2006Kemp 2006; however, studies are only just beginning to examine in detail how morphology maps to orthography in the development of orthographic representations. For example, Burani et al (2008) found that children were faster to read psuedowords that were made up of root and suffix morphemes (for example, womanist is a pseudoword but is made from two morphemes, namely womanþist) than pseudowords that did not contain embedded morphemes. Interestingly, younger children (and poorer readers) also showed a processing advantage for words that contained morphological structure, suggesting that they were relying on morphological parsing to a greater extent than more skilled readers (see Reichle & Perfetti (2003) and Verhoeven & Perfetti (2003) for further preliminary work in this area).…”
Section: Form -Meaning Connections and Learning To Readmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, it is suggested that a reading intervention should have an additional focus on teaching "sub-word 29 orthographic-phonological connections relating to larger units" (Stainthorp, 2014, p. 2). Third, the previous studies with sublexical items, such as consonant clusters , syllables , and morphemes (Burani, Marcolini, De Luca, & Zoccolotti, 2008), have been shown to be effective in promoting reading skill in transparent languages, and such efforts have also been reported to be fruitful in English (for a review, see Stainthorp, 2014).…”
Section: 1 Reading Fluency Intervention In Transparent Orthographiesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, it is suggested that a reading intervention should have an additional focus on teaching "sub-word 29 orthographic-phonological connections relating to larger units" (Stainthorp, 2014, p. 2). Third, the previous studies with sublexical items, such as consonant clusters , syllables , and morphemes (Burani, Marcolini, De Luca, & Zoccolotti, 2008), have been shown to be effective in promoting reading skill in transparent languages, and such efforts have also been reported to be fruitful in English (for a review, see Stainthorp, 2014).One of the best candidates proposed for reading fluency training in Finnish is the syllable, justified for instance by the polysyllabic nature and clear syllable structure of the language (see also the introduction in . It is also proposed that multisyllabic words, or nonwords that cannot be recognized as a whole, be analyzed and broken down into syllables (multiple-trace memory model; Ans, Carbonnel, & Valdois, 1998), which supports the use of syllables as training units in languages characterized by polysyllabic words.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…An implicit awareness of morphology should enable more efficient processing during reading (since number markers are redundant), while an explicit understanding will improve writing by enabling deaf children to prevent and to correct mistakes appropriately. Previous intervention studies have demonstrated the effectiveness of morphological awareness training on the literacy skills of hearing children (Nunes, Bryant, & Olsson, 2003) including those with dyslexia and SLI (Burani, Marcolini, De Luca, & Zoccolotti, 2008;Elbro & Arnbak, 1996;Pawlowska, Leonard, Camarata, Brown, & Camarata, 2008;Tsesmeli & Seymour, 2009). …”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%