2017
DOI: 10.1007/978-3-319-53646-0_15
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Morphological Profile of Williams Syndrome: Typical or Atypical?

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

2
8
0
1

Year Published

2018
2018
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(11 citation statements)
references
References 46 publications
2
8
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“… Losh et al (2000) , also using regression analyses, found that WS children performed at non-verbal mental age levels in the “Frog story.” They reported that CA had effects in increasing the length of narratives but not in reducing morphological errors. In previous studies, we also observed the independence of morphological errors from verbal and CA in spontaneous speech ( Diez-Itza et al, 2017 ), but individuals with WS scored at verbal-age in narrative productivity ( Shiro et al, 2016 ). Similar results concerning the length of the stories in number of propositions and utterances had been already reported for English-speaking and French-speaking school-age children where the stories of WS participants were longer than those of DS controls but comparable to mental-age matched TD controls ( Reilly et al, 1990 ; Lacroix et al, 2007 ).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 59%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“… Losh et al (2000) , also using regression analyses, found that WS children performed at non-verbal mental age levels in the “Frog story.” They reported that CA had effects in increasing the length of narratives but not in reducing morphological errors. In previous studies, we also observed the independence of morphological errors from verbal and CA in spontaneous speech ( Diez-Itza et al, 2017 ), but individuals with WS scored at verbal-age in narrative productivity ( Shiro et al, 2016 ). Similar results concerning the length of the stories in number of propositions and utterances had been already reported for English-speaking and French-speaking school-age children where the stories of WS participants were longer than those of DS controls but comparable to mental-age matched TD controls ( Reilly et al, 1990 ; Lacroix et al, 2007 ).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 59%
“…The participants had been matched in previous studies to different samples of 5-year-old typically developing children on the basis of MLU as an indicator of verbal age. In one study of spontaneous conversation ( Diez-Itza et al, 2017 ) the TD group had a mean age of 5;5 (range: 5;0–5;11), and a mean MLUw of 4,8 (range: 2;6–9;0). In another study of narratives in conversation ( Shiro et al, 2016 ), the TD group had a mean age of 5;8 (range: 5;4–6;5), and a mean MLUw of 6,6 (range: 4;7–10;3).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The existence of within-domain dissociations within the linguistic domain in WS, as well as specific 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.992512 developmental trajectories and atypical features, especially in the case of morphology, has been widely discussed (Karmiloff-Smith et al, 1997;Karmiloff-Smith, 1998;Diez-Itza et al, 2017). Phonological development provides a better example of emergent complexity, i.e., the changing nature of a complex system over time, revealing principles and milestones across languages (Davis and Bedore, 2013;McLeod and Crowe, 2018).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In contrast, structural aspects of language have been described as relative strengths in the WS linguistic profile. Morphosyntactic abilities had been considered selectively spared ( Clahsen et al, 2004 ), although this assumption was challenged in several studies indicating some degree of atypical morphological processing ( Thomas et al, 2001 ; Boloh and Ibernon, 2010 ; Benítez-Burraco et al, 2017 ; Diez-Itza et al, 2017 ). Receptive vocabulary is also an area of relative strength in people with Down syndrome, but only for concrete vocabulary ( Mervis and John, 2008 ; Garayzábal et al, 2014 ; Moraleda and López, 2020 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Several studies have tried to describe a linguistic profile more open to individual variability (López-Rangel, Maurice, McGillivray, & Friedman, 1992), although most seem to agree on the difficulty of grammatical aspects, lexical-semantic and pragmatic respectively (Clahsen & Almazan, 1998;Levy & Bechar, 2003;Pinheiro, Galdo-Álvarez, Sampaio, Niznikiewicz, & Gonçalves, 2010;Robinson & Temple, 2009). For example, at the morphosyntactic level, children with WS have difficulties in the use of prepositions, concordances of time, gender and number, a regulated structure and an average emission length lower than expected due to their chronological age (Benítez-Burraco, Garayzábal, & Cuetos, 2016;Diez-Itza, Martínez, Fernández-Urquiza, & Antón, 2017;Garayzábal, Sotillo, & Campos, 2001;Grant, Valian, & Karmiloff-Smith, 2002). The pragmatic component also seems to have certain particularities, especially as regards communicative exchanges.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%