2021
DOI: 10.31234/osf.io/7hfm5
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

MOSAIC+: a cross-linguistic model of verb-marking in typically developing children and children with Developmental Language Disorder

Abstract: This study extends an existing cross-linguistic model of verb-marking error in children’s early multi-word speech (MOSAIC) by adding a novel mechanism that defaults to the most frequent form of the verb where this accounts for a high proportion of forms in the input. Our simulations show that the resulting dual-factor model not only provides a better explanation of the data on typically developing (TD) children, but also captures the cross-linguistic pattern of verb-marking error in children with Developmental… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2023
2023
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
3

Relationship

0
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 3 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 25 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The production pressures and the resulting compensatory behavior may result in comprehension–production dissociation in mapping forms to meanings (Gershkoff‐Stowe & Smith, 1997; Kapatsinski & Harmon, 2017; Leonard & Dispaldro, 2013; Naigles & Gelman, 1995, see also Barak et al., in revision, for computational modeling of this phenomenon). In this way, accessibility‐driven compensation explains defaulting to high‐frequency forms that has been shown to be influential in accounting for cross‐linguistic differences in morphosyntactic problems observed in children with DLD (e.g., Freudenthal et al., 2021a, 2021b).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The production pressures and the resulting compensatory behavior may result in comprehension–production dissociation in mapping forms to meanings (Gershkoff‐Stowe & Smith, 1997; Kapatsinski & Harmon, 2017; Leonard & Dispaldro, 2013; Naigles & Gelman, 1995, see also Barak et al., in revision, for computational modeling of this phenomenon). In this way, accessibility‐driven compensation explains defaulting to high‐frequency forms that has been shown to be influential in accounting for cross‐linguistic differences in morphosyntactic problems observed in children with DLD (e.g., Freudenthal et al., 2021a, 2021b).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The MOSAIC model has also been used to study the mixed productivity period (Freudenthal et al., 2006, 2010, 2023). MOSAIC is a distributional learning model with strong utterance‐final position bias.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Most previous models of morphology acquisition have focused on capturing (1) the cognitive mechanism that allows for limited generalization, that is, applying the past tense –ed to regular forms and not for irregular forms and/or (2) a possible stage of overgeneralization in which irregular verbs are occasionally produced with the regular inflection, for example, goed (Corkery, Matusevych, & Goldwater, 2019; Cottrell & Plunkett, 1994; Hare & Elman, 1995; Hoeffner, 1992; Kirov & Cotterell, 2018; Legate & Yang, 2007; MacWhinney & Leinbach, 1991; O'Donnell, 2015; Pinker & Prince, 1988; Plunkett & Juola, 1999; Rumelhart & McClelland, 1986; Yang, 2016). Those that have tried to capture the variable production of the regular –ed suffix and the bare form have either assumed that children's grammars contain incorrect form‐meaning mappings, that is that they erroneously associate past tense to bare forms (Legate & Yang, 2007) or have made predictions about form production without distinguishing whether or not children are trying to specifically produce past tense meanings in these cases (Freudenthal, Gobet, & Pine, 2023; Freudenthal, Pine, & Gobet, 2010, 2006; Freudenthal, Pine, Jones, & Gobet, 2015; Freudenthal, Ramscar, Leonard, & Pine, 2021).…”
Section: The Acquisition Of Past Tense Regular Formmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The usage‐based turn in functionalism led to an explosion of interest in frequency effects, with frequency understood as estimating the number of episodes of a particular type of experience. However, until recently, usage‐based linguists have not produced explicit models of learning (though see Freudenthal, Pine, & Gobet, 2006, 2023; McCauley & Christiansen, 2019).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%