2018
DOI: 10.1146/annurev-vision-091517-034328
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Motion Perception: From Detection to Interpretation

Abstract: Visual motion processing can be conceptually divided into two levels. In the lower level, local motion signals are detected by spatiotemporal-frequency-selective sensors and then integrated into a motion vector flow. Although the model based on V1-MT physiology provides a good computational framework for this level of processing, it needs to be updated to fully explain psychophysical findings about motion perception, including complex motion signal interactions in the spatiotemporal-frequency and space domains… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
5

Citation Types

1
27
0
1

Year Published

2019
2019
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
3
1

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 38 publications
(29 citation statements)
references
References 143 publications
1
27
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…One way to quantify the dynamic deformation is to measure the movement pattern through analyzing features of the optical flow fields extracted from the videos. Previous research suggests that humans could use distortions in the optical flow fields to estimate mechanical properties (e.g., softness) of an object (Bi, Jin, Nienborg, & Xiao, 2018;Kawabe, Maruya, Fleming, & Nishida, 2015;Nishida, Kawabe, Sawayama, & Fukiage, 2018;Paulun et al, 2017;van Assen, Citation: Bi, W., Jin, P., Nienborg, H., & Xiao, B. (2019).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One way to quantify the dynamic deformation is to measure the movement pattern through analyzing features of the optical flow fields extracted from the videos. Previous research suggests that humans could use distortions in the optical flow fields to estimate mechanical properties (e.g., softness) of an object (Bi, Jin, Nienborg, & Xiao, 2018;Kawabe, Maruya, Fleming, & Nishida, 2015;Nishida, Kawabe, Sawayama, & Fukiage, 2018;Paulun et al, 2017;van Assen, Citation: Bi, W., Jin, P., Nienborg, H., & Xiao, B. (2019).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…More specifically, we synthesise the available evidence on the physiological drivers and signatures of movement. As our aim is to link this condition-dependence to ecology, we do not review the current neurobiological basis of movement decisions as in [13][14][15][16], nor the physiology behind wing development in insects [17,18] but instead provide a synthesis on (1) how physiological state as measured in its most coarse way by body condition correlates with movement decisions related to foraging, migration and dispersal, (2) how changes in stress hormones underlie changes in these movement strategies and (3) whether these can be related to alternative Fig. 1 Setting the scene.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Furthermore, recognizing materials in natural environments likely entails integrating spatiotemporally segregated information into coherent percepts, similar to when detecting animate objects (think of detecting a tiger through long, swaying grass). Given that many brain regions are sensitive to certain kinds of motion structure (for reviews see Erlikhman et al, 2018;Kourtzi et al, 2008;Nishida et al, 2018), it is surprising that only very few studies have investigated the neural mechanisms involved in material perception using dynamic stimuli (but see Okazawa et al 2012;Kam et al 2015;see Sun et al 2016a for binocular stimuli). These studies investigated the neural mechanisms of gloss perception using rigid objects.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%