2017
DOI: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2016.05.008
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Mouse strain differences in punished ethanol self-administration

Abstract: Determining the neural factors contributing to compulsive behaviors such as alcohol-use disorders (AUDs) has become a significant focus of current preclinical research. Comparison of phenotypic differences across genetically distinct mouse strains provides one approach to identify molecular and genetic factors contributing to compulsive-like behaviors. Here we examine a rodent assay for punished ethanol self-administration in four widely used inbred strains known to differ on ethanol-related behaviors: C57BL/6… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
14
1

Year Published

2017
2017
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

3
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 22 publications
(16 citation statements)
references
References 93 publications
1
14
1
Order By: Relevance
“…We first examined the behavioral profile of mice undergoing in vivo neuronal recordings. Replicating previous observations (37)(38)(39), the rate of EtOH lever pressing was significantly suppressed during punishment and probe testing, compared with the unpunished baseline period of the punishment session ( Figure 1B, C). The number of shocks received inversely predicted suppression on probe (i.e., positive correlation between EtOH-SA during punishment and probe [r = .61, p = .003]), showing that the degree of suppression during probe testing was not due to the number of shocks received.…”
Section: Punishment Suppresses Etoh-sasupporting
confidence: 89%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…We first examined the behavioral profile of mice undergoing in vivo neuronal recordings. Replicating previous observations (37)(38)(39), the rate of EtOH lever pressing was significantly suppressed during punishment and probe testing, compared with the unpunished baseline period of the punishment session ( Figure 1B, C). The number of shocks received inversely predicted suppression on probe (i.e., positive correlation between EtOH-SA during punishment and probe [r = .61, p = .003]), showing that the degree of suppression during probe testing was not due to the number of shocks received.…”
Section: Punishment Suppresses Etoh-sasupporting
confidence: 89%
“…Mice were trained in a Med Associates operant chamber (Med Associates, Inc., Fairfax, VT) to respond on the left of two levers on a continuous schedule of reinforcement to earn sucrose pellets during 40-minute sessions (the right, "inactive" lever had no programed consequences) until the criterion was met ($35 rewards), as previously described (37)(38)(39). The pellet was then substituted with a progression of 10-mL liquid rewards ("sucrose fading"; 10% sucrose, 10% sucrose 1 10% EtOH, 5% sucrose 1 10% EtOH, 10% EtOH).…”
Section: Behavioral Testingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Multiple measures of operant responding for food reward were assessed following CIE. Mice were first given CIE exposure for 4 weeks and operant testing began 3 days after the completion of CIE exposure (for schematic of experimental design, see Fig. A ) and continued for 5 to 6 weeks using methods previously described (Halladay et al., ; Radke et al., , ). Mice were maintained at ~85% of their free‐feeding body weight throughout testing to motivate performance.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While the active-hole responding exhibited by 129S1 and 129X1 mice during early training was superimposable, 129S1 mice exhibited very poor hole discrimination (Figs. Further, 129S1 can be trained to respond for food pellet, sucrose and alcohol reinforcement (Halladay et al 2017), arguing intact operant-conditioning processes in this substrain. The poor hole discrimination exhibited by 129S1 mice does not likely reflect some general deficit in reward processing or operant-learning as 129S1 mice exhibit: unconditioned approach towards appetitive, nondrug reinforcers similarly to other 129-unrelated isogenic mouse strains (Malkesman et al 2010) and comparable voluntary intake of very low-dose oxycodone and fentanyl vs. the other 129 substrains (Fig.…”
Section: Substrain Differences In Oral Oxycodone Reinforcementmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…1). Further, 129S1 can be trained to respond for food pellet, sucrose and alcohol reinforcement (Halladay et al 2017), arguing intact operant-conditioning processes in this substrain. Thus, future studies will examine for the generalization of phenotype across different oxycodone training-doses, as well as across other drug and nondrug reinforcers to probe the opioid-specificity of their low oxycodone reinforcement phenotype.…”
Section: Substrain Differences In Oral Oxycodone Reinforcementmentioning
confidence: 99%