2017
DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2017.02.005
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Multi-group and hierarchical confirmatory factor analysis of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Fifth Edition: What does it measure?

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

11
81
1
2

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
1
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 76 publications
(95 citation statements)
references
References 61 publications
11
81
1
2
Order By: Relevance
“…However, one study (H. Chen, Zhang, Raiford, Zhu, & Weiss, ) reported factorial invariance of the final publisher preferred WISC‐V higher‐order model with five‐group factors across gender, although it did not examine invariance for rival higher‐order or bifactor models. Likewise, Reynolds and Keith () reported WISC‐V invariance across age groups, but the model they examined for invariance was an oblique five‐factor model, which ignores general intelligence altogether.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, one study (H. Chen, Zhang, Raiford, Zhu, & Weiss, ) reported factorial invariance of the final publisher preferred WISC‐V higher‐order model with five‐group factors across gender, although it did not examine invariance for rival higher‐order or bifactor models. Likewise, Reynolds and Keith () reported WISC‐V invariance across age groups, but the model they examined for invariance was an oblique five‐factor model, which ignores general intelligence altogether.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Psychometrically unidimensional tasks with high factor loadings on fluid intelligence also share similar patterns of associations with superior frontal, inferior and posterior parietal and temporal-occipital regions (Ebisch et al, 2012). Overall, it is not surprising that these pattern of findings are consistent with the parieto-frontal integration theory (P-FIT) of general intelligence neural correlates, given that fluid intelligence ability is related to a higher-order general intelligence factor (Carroll, 1993;Colom et al, 2009;Jung & Haier, 2007;Reynolds & Keith, 2017). The recent voxel-based meta-analysis of Basten, Hilger, and Fiebach (2015) on brain structural and functional correlates of intelligence lends further support to the P-FIT hypothesis.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 80%
“…For our study, we relied on full and abbreviated forms of the Wechsler intelligence scales with established construct validity and reliability in both typically developing and ASD populations (Minshew, Turner, & Goldstein, 2005;Scott, Austin, & Reid, 2007;Weiss, Keith, Zhu, & Chen, 2013). Others have suggested that the abbreviated form may overestimate nonverbal intelligence ability, and the PRI has also been recently separated into two independent factors representing fluid intelligence and visual processing in the latest iteration of the WISC (Axelrod, 2002;Reynolds & Keith, 2017). Varying definitions and measurement of cognitive constructs might account for inter-site differences in findings, such as the prominent occipital mediation in ASD that we observed in replication analyses.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Fluid intelligence refers to the ability to solve novel problems, and is typically estimated from composite scores of nonverbal or abstract tests. Under the Catell‐Horn‐Carroll (CHC) model, fluid intelligence (Gf) is a broad ability subsumed under an overall general intelligence factor (Reynolds & Keith, ; Schneider & McGrew, ). The autism spectrum disorders (ASD) are a group of heterogeneous neurodevelopmental conditions associated with deficits in social communication, social interaction, and restricted and repetitive behaviours.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%