Language and Concept Acquisition From Infancy Through Childhood 2020
DOI: 10.1007/978-3-030-35594-4_8
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Multiple Examples Support Children’s Word Learning: The Roles of Aggregation, Decontextualization, and Memory Dynamics

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
12
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4

Relationship

0
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(12 citation statements)
references
References 79 publications
0
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Second, in other studies with pre‐school‐age children, even incidental and irrelevant changes, such as the background color of an image in a word‐learning task, result in large effects on both memory and generalization of the words (Vlach & Sandhofer, 2011). Third, what comprises the “same context” broadens over development, such that infants and young children may be more sensitive to context changes than adults (Rovee‐Collier & Cuevas, 2009; also see Sandhofer & Schonberg, 2020). Context changes that are salient to adults may also be unlikely to be unnoticed by children.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Second, in other studies with pre‐school‐age children, even incidental and irrelevant changes, such as the background color of an image in a word‐learning task, result in large effects on both memory and generalization of the words (Vlach & Sandhofer, 2011). Third, what comprises the “same context” broadens over development, such that infants and young children may be more sensitive to context changes than adults (Rovee‐Collier & Cuevas, 2009; also see Sandhofer & Schonberg, 2020). Context changes that are salient to adults may also be unlikely to be unnoticed by children.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Fourth, what drives the semantic differentiation should be explored. Decontextualization (Lucariello, Kyratzis, & Engel, 1986;Sandhofer & Schonberg, 2020) would play a central role. Seeing shoes in various contexts (e.g., doorway or shop), or getting to know shoes used in unconventional ways (e.g., shoes as vases) might facilitate semantic differentiation.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Children around 7-or 8month-olds can segment words from continuous speech (Estes & Lew-Williams, 2015;Jusczyk & Aslin, 1995;Jusczyk, Houston, & Newsome, 1999) and such word segmentation becomes more robust to changes in speakers or speakers' emotions around the age of 10 months (Houston & Jusczyk, 2000;Schmale & Seidl, 2009;Singh, Morgan, & White, 2004). Research on word segmentation was a precursor to studies on infant statistical learning in language development (a landmark study is Saffran, Aslin, & Newport, 1996; see also Black & Bergmann, 2017;Saffran & Kirkham, 2018;Sandhofer & Schonberg, 2020).…”
Section: The Dominance Of Nouns In Early Vocabularymentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Thus, when thinking about word learning in children's daily lives, the context refers to all parts of the situation in which the learning event took place. Prior research indicates that children are more likely to aggregate information into one category when there is a greater similarity in their memories of the category instances, even when that information is not diagnostic of the learned category (Sandhofer & Schonberg, 2020). In this way, aspects of the broader context (e.g., location, person speaking) can influence word learning.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%