1999
DOI: 10.1016/s0042-6989(99)00131-5
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Multiple gain control processes in contrast–contrast phenomena

Abstract: Spatial interactions among orientation-tuned gain control processes are presumed to mediate center-surround contrast-contrast phenomena. In this paper, we assess contributions of gain control processes that pool over orientation. We measured the apparent contrast of a luminance-modulated center disk embedded in various modulated surrounds. In all conditions, observers compared the apparent contrast of the test center to an identically modulated disk with no surround. When center and surround are simple, vertic… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
35
0

Year Published

2003
2003
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 45 publications
(38 citation statements)
references
References 21 publications
3
35
0
Order By: Relevance
“…6A (right). Moreover, while the psychophysical effect of spatial phase is in line with other studies that, similar to ours, imposed no gap between the foreground and surround stimuli (Ejima and Takahashi 1985;Olzak and Laurinen 1999), this effect is absent in studies where there is a gap (Cannon and Fullenkamp 1991;Petrov and McKee 2006;Xing and Heeger 2001), suggesting the potential influence of a brightness induction effect at the border (see Snowden and Hammett 1998).…”
Section: ϫ15supporting
confidence: 92%
“…6A (right). Moreover, while the psychophysical effect of spatial phase is in line with other studies that, similar to ours, imposed no gap between the foreground and surround stimuli (Ejima and Takahashi 1985;Olzak and Laurinen 1999), this effect is absent in studies where there is a gap (Cannon and Fullenkamp 1991;Petrov and McKee 2006;Xing and Heeger 2001), suggesting the potential influence of a brightness induction effect at the border (see Snowden and Hammett 1998).…”
Section: ϫ15supporting
confidence: 92%
“…For example, pattern regularity leads to more crowding in behavioral paradigms (Manassi et al, 2012;Sayim et al, 2008) and attenuated BOLD responses to the target (Joo et al, 2012). Similarly, surround suppression is strongest, behaviorally and physiologically, when the surround is similar to the target by orientation, phase and spatial frequency (Cavanaugh et al, 2002;Olzak and Laurinen, 1999;Saarela and Herzog, 2008;Williams et al, 2003).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is our opinion that these authors' argument is only partly valid, since surround suppression does occur at the fovea when the contrast of the target is above threshold, i.e. when suppression is measured either using supra-threshold contrast matching (Cannon & Fullenkamp, 1991, 1993; Chubb, Sperling, & Solomon, 1989; Ejima & Takahashi, 1985; Kilpeläinen, Donner, & Laurinen, 2007; Nurminen, Peromaa, & Laurinen, 2010; Olzak & Laurinen, 1999; Snowden & Hammett, 1998; Solomon, Sperling, & Chubb, 1993; Xing & Heeger, 2000, 2001) or detection on a pedestal (Chen & Tyler, 2002; Snowden & Hammett, 1998; Yu & Levi, 1997) tasks. Moreover, the argument that the spatial extent of surround modulation does not scale with spatial frequency is controversial, because the spatial extent of near-surround modulation does scale at supra-threshold contrast, at least up to 4.8 cycles surround width (Cannon & Fullenkamp, 1991).…”
Section: The Multiple Components Of Surround Modulation: Multiple mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Albright & Stoner, 2002; Allman, Miezin, & Mc Guinness, 1985; Born & Bradley, 2005; Desimone & Schein, 1987; Pollen et al, 2002) and across different modalities, including the auditory (Sutter et al, 1999), somatosensory (Sachdev, Krause, & Mazer, 2012; Vega-Bermudez & Johnson, 1999) and olfactory (Olsen & Wilson, 2008) systems. In human visual perception, many studies have demonstrated that spatial context alters the perception of a visual target (Cannon & Fullenkamp, 1991; Chubb, Sperling, & Solomon, 1989; Ejima & Takahashi, 1985; Meese & Hess, 2004; Meese et al, 2007; Nurminen et al, 2009; Olzak & Laurinen, 1999; Snowden & Hammett, 1998). The conservation across such a wide range of species, cortical areas and sensory modalities suggests that surround modulation plays a fundamental role in sensory processing.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%