2017
DOI: 10.1152/jn.00408.2017
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Multiple spatial representations interact to increase reach accuracy when coordinating a saccade with a reach

Abstract: Reaching is an essential behavior that allows primates to interact with the environment. Precise reaching to visual targets depends on our ability to localize and foveate the target. Despite this, how the saccade system contributes to improvements in reach accuracy remains poorly understood. To assess spatial contributions of eye movements to reach accuracy, we performed a series of behavioral psychophysics experiments in nonhuman primates (). We found that a coordinated saccade with a reach to a remembered ta… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
1
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 63 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Reaches tend to decrease saccade latency and increase saccade velocity in both macaques and humans (Sailer et al 2016;Snyder et al 2002). Although macaques can be trained to decouple gaze fixation from reach direction (Hawkins et al 2013), accuracy and temporal coupling are higher when animals are allowed to coordinate the eye and hand toward a common goal (Vazquez et al 2017). In general, the kinematic rules for eye-hand coordination in macaques are similar to those observed in the more frequently studied human eye-hand coordination system (Van Donkelaar and Staub 2000;Henriques et al 1998;Prablanc et al 1979).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 66%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Reaches tend to decrease saccade latency and increase saccade velocity in both macaques and humans (Sailer et al 2016;Snyder et al 2002). Although macaques can be trained to decouple gaze fixation from reach direction (Hawkins et al 2013), accuracy and temporal coupling are higher when animals are allowed to coordinate the eye and hand toward a common goal (Vazquez et al 2017). In general, the kinematic rules for eye-hand coordination in macaques are similar to those observed in the more frequently studied human eye-hand coordination system (Van Donkelaar and Staub 2000;Henriques et al 1998;Prablanc et al 1979).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 66%
“…Studies of eye-hand coordination in macaques are somewhat less common than either studies of eye-head coordination in macaques or eye-hand coordination in the human. These can be grouped into studies where gaze fixation and hand motion were confined to a common twodimensional plane (Dean et al 2011;Pesaran et al 2006;Snyder et al 2002;Song and McPeek 2009;Vazquez et al 2017), versus those where animals performed a three-dimensional reach in depth (Battaglia-Mayer A Genovesio A, et al 2001;Ferraina et al 1997;Hawkins et al 2013;Marconi B Battaglia-Mayer A, et al 2001;Marzocchi et al 2008). Macaques will generally saccade in advance to capture the reach target (Pelz and Canosa 2001;Song and McPeek 2009).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Eye movements are usually made spontaneously when observers engage in visually-guided manual tasks such as reaching, grasping, pointing, or hitting. Eye and hand movements are spatially and temporally coordinated: gaze leads the hand by up to 1 s (Ballard, Hayhoe, Li, & Whitehead, 1992;Smeets et al, 1996;Sailer, Flanagan, & Johansson, 2005;Land, 2006), and gaze locations are anchored to future contact points on the target, indicating strong spatial coupling (van Donkelaar, Lee, & Gellman, 1994;Neggers & Bekkering, 2000;Gribble, Everling, Ford, & Mattar, 2002;Brenner & Smeets, 2011;Cesqui, Mezzetti, Lacquaniti, & d'Avella, 2015;Vazquez, Federici, & Pesaran, 2017). Many of these studies have focused on the saccade-toreach relationship.…”
Section: Eye Movement Training Does Not Transfer To Hand Movementsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Numerous studies have demonstrated that simultaneous eye and arm movements are coordinated both spatially and temporally, suggesting a shared representation for planning movements of the eyes and the arms (Carey, 2000;Crawford, Medendorp, & Marotta, 2004;Dean, Marti, Tsui, Rinzel, & Pesaran, 2011;Fischer & Rogal, 1986;Fisk & Goodale, 1985;Frens & Erkelens, 1991;Gribble, Everling, Ford, & Mattar, 2002;Herman, Herman, & Maulucci, 1981;Jeannerod, 1988;Land & Hayhoe, 2001;Neggers & Bekkering, 2002;Prablanc, Echallier, Komilis, & Jeannerod, 1979;Sailer, Eggert, Ditterich, & Straube, 2000;Song & McPeek, 2009). The shared representation implies that motor commands for the eyes can also influence the arm, and vice versa (Lee, Poizner, Corcos, & Henriques, 2014;Sailer, Eggert, Ditterich, & Straube, 2002;Soechting, Engel, & Flanders, 2001;Vazquez, Federici, & Pesaran, 2017). There are other studies that show little or no correlations between the timings (onsets) of eye and arm movements, suggesting independent representations for the different effectors (Guitton & Volle, 1987;Tweed, Glenn, & Vilis, 1995;Vercher, Magenes, Prablanc, & Gauthier, 1994).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%