2016
DOI: 10.1037/pmu0000138
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Musical listening and kinesthesis: Is there an audio-vocal tuning system?

Abstract: Kinesthesis, the sense of muscular effort that accompanies bodily movement, is known to be important in musical performance. Less well understood is the role of the kinesthetic sense in musical listening. Recent observations that listening to music is associated with fast, subtle, pitch-related patterns of kinesthetic sensations that involve the ears, eustachian tubes, nasopharynx, vocal tract, and even muscles of facial expression challenges traditional accounts of auditory processing divorced from peripheral… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

1
4
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 116 publications
(220 reference statements)
1
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This prediction is strengthened by the finding that hearing music is experienced as rewarding (Mas-Herrero, Marco-Pallares, Lorenzo-Seva, Zatorre, & Rodriguez-Fornells, 2013; Menon & Levitin, 2005), which, we assume, can positively reinforce “hearing” music (i.e., imagining it). It is further supported by findings of overlapping neurological substrates between perception and imagery (Herholz, Halpern, & Zatorre, 2012) and studies indicating the presence of imagery or internal mimicry during music perception (Miller, 2016; Zatorre, Chen, & Penhune, 2007). Because music can act as a reward and is at the same time combined with other activities, a process of conditioning takes place, which goes beyond that of a simple association process: The conditioned stimulus (activity) that is paired with a rewarding (unconditioned) stimulus (the music) causes a (unconditioned) response (INMI).…”
Section: Relations Between Music Listening Habits and Inmisupporting
confidence: 60%
“…This prediction is strengthened by the finding that hearing music is experienced as rewarding (Mas-Herrero, Marco-Pallares, Lorenzo-Seva, Zatorre, & Rodriguez-Fornells, 2013; Menon & Levitin, 2005), which, we assume, can positively reinforce “hearing” music (i.e., imagining it). It is further supported by findings of overlapping neurological substrates between perception and imagery (Herholz, Halpern, & Zatorre, 2012) and studies indicating the presence of imagery or internal mimicry during music perception (Miller, 2016; Zatorre, Chen, & Penhune, 2007). Because music can act as a reward and is at the same time combined with other activities, a process of conditioning takes place, which goes beyond that of a simple association process: The conditioned stimulus (activity) that is paired with a rewarding (unconditioned) stimulus (the music) causes a (unconditioned) response (INMI).…”
Section: Relations Between Music Listening Habits and Inmisupporting
confidence: 60%
“…Students also may draw on kinesthetic faculties when listening and encoding during dictation (Buonviri, 2014a; Mikumo, 1994; Thompson, 2004). None of the participants in the current study exhibited overtly visible signs of kinesthetic strategies (e.g., silent trumpet fingerings or cello left-hand position), but research suggests that kinesthesia can play a role in music perception in much more subtle ways (Miller, 2016). It is possible that participants silently engaged vocal apparatus kinesthesia while listening or processing in Condition 1 (Lévêque & Schön, 2015; Miller, 2016).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 68%
“…None of the participants in the current study exhibited overtly visible signs of kinesthetic strategies (e.g., silent trumpet fingerings or cello left-hand position), but research suggests that kinesthesia can play a role in music perception in much more subtle ways (Miller, 2016). It is possible that participants silently engaged vocal apparatus kinesthesia while listening or processing in Condition 1 (Lévêque & Schön, 2015; Miller, 2016). However, given their relatively inaccurate singing performance in Condition 3, one would assume that such kinesthetic engagement would affect their scores negatively in Condition 1 (silence), especially because decreasing audible feedback appears to decrease singing accuracy (Beck, Rieser, & Erdemir, 2017).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 68%
“…In Experiment 3, we ruled out that the observed effects were influenced by differences between motor learning and reading (Schiavio & Timmer, 2016), as the additional motor component affected neither recognition performance nor either of the metamemory judgments. Thus, despite the importance of kinaesthetic considerations in music processing in general (Miller, 2016), they did not seem to matter in the current study. We should note that any motor representation can indeed affect music processing (Leman, Maes, Nijs, & Van Dyck, 2018), and what the fingers, hands, and arms do during playing can act as additional memory or metamemory cues.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 56%