2010
DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.10.019
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Neural activation in arousal and reward areas of the brain in day-active and night-active grass rats

Abstract: In the diurnal unstriped Nile grass rat (Arvicanthis niloticus) access to a running wheel can trigger a shift in active phase preference, with some individuals becoming night-active (NA), while others continue to be day-active (DA). To investigate the contributions of different neural systems to the support of this shift in locomotor activity, we investigated the association between chronotype and Fos expression during the day and night in three major nuclei in the basal forebrain (BF) cholinergic (ACh) arousa… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

3
28
0

Year Published

2011
2011
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 21 publications
(31 citation statements)
references
References 57 publications
(91 reference statements)
3
28
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Although species differences may be responsible for these divergent outcomes, these observations nevertheless support the view that voluntary reversals of the phase of activity (present study) affect the brain and the circadian system in a fashion that differs from the effects of forced-activity paradigms (Karatsoreos et al, 2011, McDonald et al, 2013, Saderi et al, 2013, Hsieh et al, 2014). In agreement with this view are the results of direct comparisons of the effects on the brain of spontaneous and forced wakefulness (Castillo-Ruiz et al, 2010, Castillo-Ruiz and Nunez, 2011). Thus, in grass rats, voluntary nocturnal wakefulness results in increased neuronal activation, as indicated by Fos expression, in reward areas of the brain (i.e., horizontal diagonal band, ventral tegmental area, and supramammillary nuclei; Castillo-Ruiz et al,2010), which is an observation not extended to studies using a forced-wakefulness paradigm with the same species (Castillo-Ruiz and Nunez, 2011) The NA grass rat appears to be an attractive model to understand the consequences of activity during the rest phase in a diurnal species, without the effects of the stress associated with forced-wakefulness paradigms (Castillo-Ruiz and Nunez, 2011).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 64%
“…Although species differences may be responsible for these divergent outcomes, these observations nevertheless support the view that voluntary reversals of the phase of activity (present study) affect the brain and the circadian system in a fashion that differs from the effects of forced-activity paradigms (Karatsoreos et al, 2011, McDonald et al, 2013, Saderi et al, 2013, Hsieh et al, 2014). In agreement with this view are the results of direct comparisons of the effects on the brain of spontaneous and forced wakefulness (Castillo-Ruiz et al, 2010, Castillo-Ruiz and Nunez, 2011). Thus, in grass rats, voluntary nocturnal wakefulness results in increased neuronal activation, as indicated by Fos expression, in reward areas of the brain (i.e., horizontal diagonal band, ventral tegmental area, and supramammillary nuclei; Castillo-Ruiz et al,2010), which is an observation not extended to studies using a forced-wakefulness paradigm with the same species (Castillo-Ruiz and Nunez, 2011) The NA grass rat appears to be an attractive model to understand the consequences of activity during the rest phase in a diurnal species, without the effects of the stress associated with forced-wakefulness paradigms (Castillo-Ruiz and Nunez, 2011).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 64%
“…Orexin neurons appear to synapse directly on basal forebrain cholinergic neurons important in cognition (Castillo-Ruiz et al, 2010; Fadel et al, 2005; Frederick-Duus et al, 2007), and orexin appears to mediate long-term potentiation in the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus (Akbari et al, 2011). Prior work utilizing operant tasks shows that supplementing orexin might affect cognitive processes (Choi et al, 2010; Sharf et al, 2010; Thorpe et al, 2005).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…1A). Finally, a 160 μm 2 sampling box was placed in the caudal SUM, as described previously (Castillo-Ruiz et al, 2010). …”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The mDR, MR, and SUM did not require this procedure since these regions extended across the midline (see Fig. 1 and Castillo-Ruiz et al, 2010). In addition, for the lDR and all levels of the mDR cell counts were divided by the area occupied by their sampling box and the results are expressed as number of cells/mm 2 .…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%