2016
DOI: 10.1016/j.bandl.2015.10.007
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Neurobiological evidence for voicing underspecification in English

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
5

Citation Types

12
72
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 46 publications
(84 citation statements)
references
References 53 publications
12
72
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Multiple tokens of the deviant and standard stimuli provide this within category variation. MMN responses from a high token-variability paradigm can lead to a different pattern of results than found from a paradigm in which only one token of standard and one of a deviant stimulus are used, because the single token paradigm allows for discrimination solely on the basis of acoustic difference (Hestvik and Durvasula, 2016). In addition, increased phonological complexity of stimuli is likely to lead to greater reliance on the phonological level of processing.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Multiple tokens of the deviant and standard stimuli provide this within category variation. MMN responses from a high token-variability paradigm can lead to a different pattern of results than found from a paradigm in which only one token of standard and one of a deviant stimulus are used, because the single token paradigm allows for discrimination solely on the basis of acoustic difference (Hestvik and Durvasula, 2016). In addition, increased phonological complexity of stimuli is likely to lead to greater reliance on the phonological level of processing.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In this paper, we turn to neurophysiological data, in the form of the Mismatch Negativity (MMN) paradigm, that has been argued to reveal at least some aspects of phonological structure (Phillips et al, 2000; Walter and Hacquard, 2004; Kazanina et al, 2006; Scharinger et al, 2010, 2012; Cornell et al, 2011, 2013; Law et al, 2013; Truckenbrodt et al, 2014; de Jonge and Boersma, 2015; Hestvik and Durvasula, 2016; Politzer-Ahles et al, 2016; Schluter et al, 2016) in order to test these different representational approaches. In three MMN experiments, we test English, Arabic, and Russian, three different languages that have a functional two-way voicing distinction at a phonological level, but which rely on different underlying articulatory mechanisms to implement these distinctions during speech production.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moreover, Vaux (1998) argues that, cross-linguistically, it is the voiceless fricative that is marked, except in languages like Burmese which contrast voiced /z/, voiceless /s/, and voiceless aspirated /s h / fricatives. Recent neurophysiological evidence, however, has been argued to support the laryngeal realism hypothesis (Hestvik and Durvasula, 2016). …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations